Ethics Article Reviews

A good article review will include the following:

  • A ½ page (minimum) review of the main ideas the article discusses and
  • A ½ page (minimum) personal reflection on the article consisting of your personal opinions, thoughts, and/or experiences related to the topic

Your complete assignment will be 1 page per article, 2 pages total. Each article is worth 1.5 points.

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Ethics Article Reviews
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

J Bus Ethics (2011) 99:39–48

DOI 10.1007/s10551-011-1165-6

Substantive Ethics: Integrating Law and Ethics in Corporate
Ethics Programs

Mark S. Blodget

t

Published online: 24 January 2012

� Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Abstract Continual corporate malfeasance signals the

need for obeying the law and for enhancing business ethics

perspectives. Yet, the relationship between law and ethics

and its integrative role in defining values are often unclear.

While integrity-based ethics programs emphasize ethics

values more than law or compliance, viewing ethics as

being integrated with law may enhance understanding of a

n

organization’s core values. The author refers to this inte-

gration of law and ethics as ‘‘substantive ethics,’’ analo-

gous to the substantive law that evolves over time, which is

applied to ethical breaches and carries sanctions for non-

compliance. This article describes the integration of law

and ethics as a mid-point between two polar views that

define law and ethics either as having no relation or as

being one and the same. Since corporations expressly state

which laws they follow, a sample of corporate compliance

statements is used to demonstrate this integrative mid-

point. The sample also reveals that corporate ethics codes

rarely express ethics and law as being integrated per se.

Therefore, the author creates an example of a securities law

compliance statement that is introduced with an integrative

perspective of law and ethics. Perhaps such revised cor-

porate codes will encourage corporate respect for both law

and ethics and enhance ethical sustainability.

Keywords Business ethics and law � Core values �
Corporate ethics codes � Ethical sustainability �
Integrity-based corporate ethics programs �
Substantive ethics

M. S. Blodgett (&)

Business Law & Ethics, Sawyer Business School,

Suffolk University, Boston, MA 02108-2770, USA

e-mail: mblodget@suffolk.edu

‘‘Obeying the law, both in letter and in spirit, is the

foundation on which this Company’s ethical stan-

dards are built’’ (Avery 2009).

Introduction

Business ethics controversies continue to arise in an era of

global economic crisis, egregious corporate malfeasance,

exorbitant CEO compensation, fraudulent investment

schemes, proliferation of tort suits, and global human rights

abuses. While protection of the economy, investors, and

other stakeholders is of major concern, calls for tort caps

may reduce the parameters of responsibility. Despite the

need for greater understanding of these matters, the role of

law as it relates to these business ethics issues is not well

understood. The law is often discounted as ‘‘mere rules’’

without acknowledgment of its ethical content. Yet,

responses to crises in business ethics often include new

regulation in addition to pleas for more ethical business

behavior. The connection between law and business ethics

compels us to more fully explore its role in the current

global business environment that increasingly demands

ethical sustainability. ‘‘For globalization to succeed, it must

be ethical’’ (Hoffman and Driscoll 2000, p. 222) and

generate new business ethics perspectives.

Perhaps this role and challenge are the most prominent

within the context of corporate behavior. Thus, this article

explores the inter-relationship of law and ethics as applied

to managerial decision-making and corporate governance

by (1) explaining a current, applied approach to the inter-

relationship between law and ethics; (2) proposing that a

new way of thinking about this inter-relationship is by

‘‘re-imaging’’ the ethics content of the law as ‘‘substantive

123

mailto:mblodget@suffolk.edu

40 M. S. Blodgett

ethics,’’ analogous to the substantive law; and by (3) pro-

posing that corporate codes of ethics be revised to

expressly encompass this inter-relationship as consistent

with values or integrity-based corporate ethics programs.

An Applied Approach to Law and Ethics

Over the centuries, numerous philosophers and legal

scholars have debated the relationship between law and

ethics. However, it is not the intent of this author to review

either ancient philosophy or the more recent exchanges of

Hart (1958) and Fuller’s (1958) debate on their relation-

ship, or to revive the past philosophical discourse of

luminaries as enumerated without elaboration in Dunfee

and Paine’s similar approaches to this topic (1996; 1994a,

b). Each accepts such a connection and applies it to busi-

ness ethics issues. Nor does the author address unethical or

ethically neutral law. As Herrera tells us, an exhaustive

answer to the relationship between law and ethics is

complicated and leads only to further inquiry. He points

out that ‘‘entire books will continue to be written on the

philosophical linkage between law and ethics from the

utilitarian perspective, the Kantian perspective, or the

Natural-law perspective’’ (Herrera 2000, p. 2).

Rather, this article draws upon the scholarly tradition

that recognizes a connection between law and ethics, an

enduring philosophical interest (Dunfee 1996) that is

foundational to contemporary business and legal issues.

Law and ethics do not conflict, nor are they mutually

exclusive (Schwartz et al. 2005). As Rawls tells us, the law

has exerted considerable influence upon philosophical

thought through its evolution, principles, and precedent

(Dunfee 1996). Therefore, the author addresses this inte-

grated or ‘‘coinciding’’ view of law and ethics (Paine

1994a, b, p. 165) in the section below to further enhance

understanding of the role of law within the application of

business ethics today.

Coinciding View

Where there is integration of law and ethics, the ethical

purpose of the law should be evident; for example, laws

prohibiting fraud enhance the ethics of disclosure and

fairness and in this example, law and ethics can be said to

coincide. Yet, perhaps it is more helpful elaborating on this

coinciding view of law and ethics by thinking of it as a

middle ground existing between two polar extremes: (1)

where law and ethics are considered to be totally separate,

and (2) where law and ethics are considered to be identical.

Although at times law and ethics are inter-related, many

continue to differentiate sharply between law and ethics,

and statements relegating law to a threshold status are

common. This ‘‘clear-line’’ approach appears in statements

that distinguish ethics from law as not only different but

also superior, such as ‘‘ethics consists of standards and

norms for behavior that are beyond laws and legal rights’’

and ‘‘ethical standards are not the standards of the law. In

fact, they are a higher standard’’ (Jennings 2006, p. 3).

While these statements contribute significantly to our

understanding of ethics and law, they may appear to

embrace the ‘‘separate realms view’’ (Paine 1994a, b,

p. 154), where law constitutes a normative minimum to

which neither ethical values attach nor from which do they

derive or evolve. Such a sharp distinction between law and

business ethics may lead to the conclusion that there is little

if any relationship between them. This supposition may

obfuscate their ‘‘coinciding’’ relationship, what Nesteruk

describes as a conduit for the law’s ‘‘morally creative role’’

that ‘‘rich interplay between law and ethics in the contem-

porary business environment’’ (Nesteruk 1999a, b, p. 604).

It is this rich inter-relationship that the American Law

Institute emphasizes as the ethical viewpoint or purpose of

the law that must be complied with rather than a narrow,

literal interpretation (Di Lorenzo 2007). As Hartman

reminds us, these rules or laws of compliance also provide

clarity to the new culture of integrity-based corporate ethics

programs (Hartman 2000). However, such lofty perceptions

do not often prevail when laws are looked upon as man-

datory compliance or as ‘‘mere rules to be followed.’’

To further enhance our perspective on this inter-rela-

tionship between law and ethics, it is necessary to identify

another viewpoint, namely, that law and ethics are in

complete harmony (the ‘‘correspondence view’’) (Paine

1994a, b, p. 156). However, this view precludes much

ethical responsibility and discretion. Each viewpoint of

separate realms and correspondence has its adherents;

however, there is a middle ground as Dunfee tells us, ‘‘the

two domains [law and ethics] are synergistically and inti-

mately related’’ and ‘‘neither can be fully meaningful or

realized without the other’’ (1996, p. 319). Hosmer and

Ehrlich deduce that, although not complete, there is con-

siderable overlap between law and ethics (2008; 2005).

This deduction leads Shaw (1996) to conclude that,

although insufficient to establish an organization’s values,

law evolves with society’s view of morals which it codifies

along with ideals. Perhaps a closer examination of these

codified morals and ideals will reveal some values suitable

for a corporation’s core values.

This considerable overlap forms an intersecting point

between the ‘‘separate realms view’’ and the ‘‘correspon-

dence view.’’ Identifying and exploring this coinciding or

integrative aspect of law and business ethics is important to

understanding a business environment that is often guided

by corporate compliance-based programs, but frequently

123

41

Substantive Ethics

engulfed in egregious breaches of both law and business

ethics. Let not the ‘‘incompleteness’’ distract us from the

mutuality of law and ethics. Separating law from business

ethics does not enhance our understanding of ethical

behavior or corporate malfeasance. Corporate compliance

statements of law carry tangible civil and/or criminal

sanctions for violations, and when sanctions are imposed,

there occurs enforcement of any coinciding business ethics

values. An emphasis on aspiration ethics may assume,

imply, and even demand another layer of responsible and

ethical business behavior, and when this occurs, it is

laudable; however, experience tells us more, and perhaps

this is the compelling reason for re-aligning and enhancing

perspectives on the coinciding view where law and ethics

are at times ‘‘synergistically and intimately related.’’

It is clear that corporations have a duty to obey the law,

yet unfortunately there are business executives who may

ignore this duty, be unaware of it, not understand it, or be

apprehensive of it. Hence, the need for compliance state-

ments, training, and broad-based communication and edu-

cation regarding the law. Ethically integrated compliance

statements imbued with express ethical perspectives may

more effectively guide behavior than narrow statements of

rules and obligations that are juxtaposed with corporate

values. For example, compliance statements prohibiting

fraud reveal the ethical perspective that intentional mis-

representation obviates good faith and honesty. This results

in a lack of trust, which should be avoided when doing

business, whether regulated or not. Integrating such ethics

values into compliance statements will unambiguously

allow the law’s ‘‘morally creative role’’ to more fully

participate in fashioning corporate ethics codes.

Corporate Ethics Codes

Analyzing corporate ethics codes is worthwhile in a global

business environment where ethical issues demand inves-

tigation (Donaldson and Werhane 1993) and where orga-

nizational culture is reflected in these formal statements

(Trevino et al. 1999). As part of corporate culture, ethics

codes contain statements of law and ethics that are self-

selected expressions or values of the corporation that are

also an identified source of

business ethics (Schwartz 2005).

Ethics codes are common among large firms (Donaldson

and Werhane 1993), and more than 90% currently use them

(Murphy 2005). They aid in identifying expectations

(Davies, ed. 1977) and provide guidance to managerial

decision-making by establishing ethical standards (Peppas

2002). According to Webley (Davies, ed. 1977), Cavanaugh

(1998) and Boatright (2003) they often include the business

ethics values of integrity, honesty, fairness, trust, and

responsibility. The last three of these values are also iden-

tified as among ‘‘six universal values’’ (Schwartz 2005).

One portion of today’s business ethics environment

centers upon corporate behavior and two approaches to

influencing this behavior through corporate ethics codes are

commonly identified as compliance-based and values or

integrity-based ethics programs. Compliance programs

focus on law and emphasize prevention and punishment. In

contrast, integrity-based programs acknowledge the law,

but are dominated by commitment to organizational values

(Trevino et al. 1999; Paine 1994a, b). Despite their differing

emphases, these two approaches are not mutually exclusive.

Approaches to corporate ethics programs have various

names and content (Weaver and Trevino 1999) spanning the

two poles of the ethics codes continuum from compliance to

integrity. Regardless of the corporate codes nomenclature,

‘‘an integrity-based program recognizes the complex rela-

tionship between law and ethics and is far more likely to

achieve the objective of responsible and effective organi-

zational conduct’’ (Paine 1994a, b, p. 169). Thus, the

identifiable and evolving inter-relationship of law and eth-

ics may reveal a new understanding and application of

business ethics that re-enforces the values or integrity-based

approach where the maximum emphasis is placed not upon

legal compliance but upon ethical decision-making.

In a 2005 comprehensive review, Schwartz compiled

and analyzed past studies of corporate ethics codes as part

of an extensive search for ‘‘universal values.’’ He also

included global codes and the writings of business ethicists,

all identified by him as sources of business ethics (Sch-

wartz 2005). His thorough review and analysis confirmed

the Aspen Declaration’s identification of six universal

values as follows: ‘‘(1) trustworthiness, (2) respect, (3)

responsibility, (4) fairness, (5) caring and (6) citizenship’’

(Schwartz 2005, p. 36). It is therefore reasonable to con-

clude that corporate ethics codes generally share in a rich

deposit of universal ethics values.

Just as corporate codes contain expressions of universal

values, they also contain legal compliance statements that

address many areas of current business concern. Many

codes blend law and ethics by varying degrees; they do not

merely juxtapose them. These ethics codes often include

expansive and global expressions of law. Because of this

expansive blend, the composition of corporate ethics codes

is worth investigating.

Methodology

Although the major or dominant types of legal compliance

statements are generally well known, commonly refer-

enced, (Post et al. 2002; Pagnatarro and Peirce 2007), and

static, the author studied a sample of 2008 Fortune 50

0

corporate ethics codes to further identify and substantiate

them and to glean additional data relevant to this study.

123

42 M. S. Blodgett

It should be, however, noted that the primary purpose of

studying this sample was not to establish the most fre-

quently occurring compliance statements per se but rather

to identify coinciding ethics values in some commonly

occurring corporate statements of legal compliance.

The sample was selected through an on-line random

number generator, and then the frequency of compliance

statements by topic was recorded, for example, ‘‘securities

laws’’ (See Exhibits 1A, 1B, 1C, 2A, and 2B). Also, state-

ments indicating the three different views—separate, cor-

responding, and coinciding, were gleaned (See Exhibit 3).

Some of the possible business ethics values coinciding with

the legal compliance areas were then identified as examples

of ‘‘substantive ethics,’’ a concept to be explained later in the

article. These examples of ‘‘substantive ethics’’ were then

corroborated with the ‘‘six universal values’’ (See Exhibit 4).

Results

The sample yielded 21 areas of corporate compliance rang-

ing in frequency from 19.35% to .81% as displayed in

Exhibit 1A 2008 Fortune 500 companies compliance data (30-

company sample)

Compliance area Term Term

frequency frequency

percentage

1 Securities 24 19.3

5

2 ‘‘All laws’’ 16 12.90

3 Anti-trust/competition 13 10.48

4 FCPA 9 7.26

5 Environment 9 7.26

6 Health and safety 9 7.26

7 Information/data 7 5.65

8 Anti-boycott 6 4.84

9 Corporate opportunity 5 4.03

10 Employment/EO/non- 5 4.03

discrimination

11 Fraud 4 3.23

12 Export law 4 3.23

13 Intellectual property 3 2.42

14 Money laundering 2 1.61

15 Bribes/Anti-Kickback Act of 1986 2 1.61

16 Immigration 1 0.81

17 Terrorism 1 0.81

18 Unfair dealing 1 0.81

19 Fair dealing 1 0.81

20 Child & forced labor 1 0.81

21 U.S. Embargo 1 0.81

Total compliance terms 1

24

Exhibits 1A, 1B 1C. Among the most prevalent areas of law

expressed in these compliance statements are as follows:

securities (30%), anti-trust (16.25%), Foreign Corrupt

Practices Act or FCPA (11.25%), environment (11.25%),

and health and safety (11.25%) (See 2A, 2B with percentages

based on a sample of 6). Four of these areas, securities

including securities fraud, FCPA, and anti-trust, track the

Federal Organizational Sentencing Guidelines for compli-

ance programs (Pagnatarro and Peirce 2007). Also prevalent

within the top 6 was ‘‘compliance with all laws’’ (20%). This

compliance statement has significance since it conceivably

includes all laws, not merely the ones specifically stated,

including U.S. law, ‘‘host countries’ laws’’ and in some

cases, ‘‘all other countries’ laws.’’ International law, a body

or system of law that exists beyond the legal system of any

particular country (Schaffer et al. 2009) may reasonably be

included in this expansive language. International law is rich

in ethics content such as the peremptory norm of jus cogens

that embodies high ideals of civilization ‘‘from which no

derogation is permitted’’ (August et al. 2009, pp. 8–9). It may

be interesting to note that no company’s ‘‘Integrity Policy’’

goes so far as to generally embrace ‘‘due care’’ (duty of care),

a legal element, and ethical value. This is consistent with past

studies of corporate ethics codes (Blodgett and Carlson

1997) unless relegated to the Board of Directors or to a

specific ‘‘professional area’’ within or tangential to the code

for accountants, lawyers, or brokers, for example.

In addition, statements among the different views—

separate, corresponding, and coinciding—yielded a variety

of ethics and law expressions along this total spectrum. The

following are representative of these expressions: ‘‘3M is

recognized worldwide as an ethical and law-abiding

company’’ and Altria’s ‘‘Our Compliance and Integrity

program is focused on doing the right thing, and reflects

our family of companies’ culture and values,’’ and Pacific

Life’s ‘‘Ethics and compliance are two closely related and

intertwining concepts that work together to help us do the

right thing’’ (2009). In general, such statements reveal

aspects of the three views—separate, corresponding, and

coinciding, but may often appear mixed and possibly

ambiguous when interpreted alone or together as in the

ethics code of one company presented below.

Since law and ethics at times coincide, it is helpful

assigning ethics values to the areas of law found in the

sample. The chart given here shows common examples of

legal compliance areas and possible coinciding ethics val-

ues that are corroborated with the ‘‘six universal values.’’

While not intended as exhaustive or complete, Exhibit 4

shows examples of numerous ethical values encompassed

by legal compliance. Many of these values are often found

in the ethics sections of corporate ethics codes—a potential

reciprocity of express ethical values with compliance

statements that may lend even more meaning to the

123

43

Sec
u

r

itie
s

“A
ll L

aw
s”

An

t

i-t

ru
st/

Com
pe

titi
on

FCPA

Env
iro

nm
en

t

Hea
lth

a
nd

S
af

et
y

In
fo

rm
at

ion

/D

at
a

Ant
i-B

oy
co

tt

Em
plo

ym
en

t/E
O/N

on
-D

isc
rim

ina
tio

n

Fra

ud

Exp
or

t L
aw

Cor
po

ra
te

O
pp

or
tu

nit
y

In
te

lle
ctu

al
Pro

pe
rty

Brib
es

/ A
nt

i-K
ick

ba
ck

A
ct

of
1

98
6

Im
m

igr
at

ion

Ter
ro

ris
m

M
on

ey
L

au
nd

er
in

g

Unf
air

D
ea

lin
g

Fair
D

ea
lin

g

Chil
d

& F
or

ce
d

La
bo

r

U.S
. E

m
ba

rg
o

Sec
ur

itie
s

“A
ll L

aw
s”

Ant
i-t

ru
st/

Com
pe

titi
on

FCPA

Env
iro

nm
en

t

Hea
lth

a
nd

S
af

et
y

In
fo

rm
at

ion
/D

at
a

Ant
i-B

oy
co

tt

Em
plo

ym
en

t/E
O/N

on
-D

isc
rim

ina
tio

n
Fra

ud

Exp
or

t L
aw

Cor
po

ra
te

O
pp

or
tu

nit
y

In
te

lle
ctu

al
Pro

pe
rty

Brib
es

/ A
nt

i-K
ick

ba
ck

A
ct

of
1

98
6

Im
m

igr
at

ion

Ter
ro

ris
m

M
on

ey
L

au
nd

er
ing

Unf
air

D
ea

lin
g

Fair
D

ea
lin

g

Chil
d

& F
or

ce
d

La
bo

r

U.S
. E

m
ba

rg
o

Sec
ur

itie
s

“A
ll L

aw
s”

Ant
i-t

ru
st/

Com
pe

titi
on

Env
iro

nm
en

t

Hea
lth

a
nd

S
af

et
y

FCPA

Substantive Ethics

24

16
13

9

9 9

7 6 5 5 4 4 3 2 2 1 1 1 1 1 1

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

T
er

m
F

re
q

u
en

cy

Compliance Term

Exhibit 1B 2008 Fortune 500 companies’ compliance areas (30-company sample)

2

5.00%

19.35%

12.90%

10.48%

7.26% 7.26% 7.26%

5.65%
4.84%

4.03% 4.03%
3.23% 3.23%

2.42%
1.61% 1.61%

0.81% 0.81% 0.81% 0.81% 0.81% 0.81%

P
er

ce
n

t
o

f
T

o
ta

l T
er

m
s

2

0.00%

15.00%

10.00%

5.00%

0.00%

Compliance Term

Exhibit 1C 2008 Fortune 500 companies’ compliance areas percentages (30-company sample)

24

16
13

9

T
er

m
F

re
q

u
en

cy 30

25

20

15

10

5

0

9 9

Compliance Term

Exhibit 2A 2008 Fortune 500 companies’ most frequent compliance areas

statement that ‘‘obeying the law, both in letter and in spirit, representative of many corporations’ stated core values, the

is the foundation on which this Company’s ethical stan- identification of which Hartman recognizes as an important

dards are built’’ (Avery 2009). Surely, these values are part of the ‘‘process of ethics integration’’ (2000, p. 157)

123

44 M. S. Blodgett

Health and Safety,
11.25%

Environment, Securities, 30.00%
11.25%

FCPA, 11.25%

Anti- All Laws , 20.00%
trust/Competition,

16.25%

Exhibit 2B 2008 Fortune 500 companies’ most frequent compliance

areas percentages

adding even more impetus to the inter-relationship of law

and ethics as an emerging perspective in ethical decision-

making and corporate governance.

Analysis: Emerging Perspectives

Many compliance areas identified in the sample are also

commonly identified in the literature (Post et al. 2002;

Pagnatarro and Peirce 2007). They refer to substantive law

Exhibit 3 Excerpts from Pacific Life, Inc. code of business conduct

that defines rights and duties (Reed et al. 2009) and they

tell much about what should be among the current ethical

concerns of business. After all, the statements were self-

selected; albeit, selected with the encouragement of the

Federal Organizational Sentencing Guidelines. Perhaps

ironically, many of the dominant legal compliance areas

are also identified as the most frequently violated laws

within firms (Post et al. 2002) and as such, their coinciding

ethics values are also violated. According to Tucker, the

federal guidelines expressly communicate that ‘‘bad ethics

is bad business’’ (Cohen 1993, p. 355). Perhaps this ethical

perspective is even more acute when viewed within a

global context since the statement ‘‘we obey all laws’’

conceivably encompasses universal ethical content and

purpose, and some areas of compliance such as ‘‘corrup-

tion’’ exist as presumed ‘‘hyper-norms’’ or fundamental

universal principles that embrace deeply held values of

humanity (Donaldson and Dunfee 1999).

Clearly, business needs a fresh outlook in managing law

and ethics. From the discussion and data presented above,

it is evident that both legal compliance and ethics are key

components of corporate governance and that reliance

Separate Coinciding Corresponding

‘‘While [ethics and compliance] serve similar ‘‘Ethics and compliance are two closely ‘‘We are committed to following laws and

functions, they are distinct enough to justify related and intertwining concepts that regulations, just as we are committed to

separate principles in our Code of Business work together to help us do the right providing excellent products and services’’

Conduct’’ thing’’

‘‘Just as there are many laws and regulations that

guide us in our compliance efforts, there are

many ethical standards that guide us in doing the

right thing’’

Exhibit 4 Compliance areas

and coinciding ethics values
Compliance area Law governing Substantive ethics values Universal values

Securities Fraud Fairness Fairness

Disclosure

Responsibility Responsibility

Insider trading

Trustworthiness Trustworthiness

Fiduciary duty

FCPA Bribes Fairness Fairness

Accounting

Anti-trust Competition Fairness Fairness

Employment Equal opportunity Caring Caring

Discrimination Fairness Fairness

Responsibility Responsibility

Trustworthiness Trustworthiness

Environment Environmental protection Caring Caring

Responsibility Responsibility

Trustworthiness Trustworthiness

123

45 Substantive Ethics

upon one may detract from the other. The currently all too

common juxtaposition of law and ethics does not serve the

best interests of ethical sustainability. Perhaps what is

necessary is a more unified view of the substantive law and

ethics.

Substantive Ethics

The argument for ethics without regard for law to guide

business behavior may be analogous to arguing for the

abolition of law and for religious teachings, philosophy,

etiquette and civility to guide our citizenry in correct

societal behavior. While this non-legal background is

necessary, highly desirable, and profound, it is also of great

importance to have laws that prohibit deceit, fraud, theft,

murder, and discrimination and that dole out substantial

penalties for violations. It is also important that society and

business respect these laws, recognizing that they embrace

fundamental ethics values.

After all, if one were to decide between laws that

‘‘coincide’’ or aspiration ethics, which would be then the

better choice: laws that embrace business ethics that are

enforced with tangible penalties, or discretionary ethical

practice that may not be intrinsically valued or acted upon

and that carries no formal incentive for doing so? If laws

carrying sanctions did not exist, how much influence would

their coinciding ethics values have in guiding managerial

decision-making?

Fortunately, there is no need to decide between law and

ethics. But this duality has been the challenge at hand

because law and ethics are more appropriately viewed as

integrative, and if not so viewed contributes to dysfunction

in corporate governance. By the late nineteenth century,

our nation’s history showed us that ‘‘[i]n a lawless world,

the exigencies of competition tended to drive commercial

practices toward the level of the most unscrupulous’’ (Hilts

2003, pp. 45–46). Hence, followed the decades of evolving

regulatory response amidst concerns for the fundamental

undermining of ‘‘faith in commercial integrity and the very

foundation of trade’’ (Hilts 2003, pp. 45–46). Conse-

quently, in our current era, substantive law makes a major

impact on ethical behavior. For example, the design,

manufacture, and distribution of products and services is a

function of the law’s standards of ‘‘duty of care.’’

Not to recognize ethics in the substantive law is to miss a

significant coinciding perspective, for our products and

services are governed as much by the ethics and law of

the ‘‘duty of care’’ as are decisions of corporate boards.

Hoffman and Driscoll (2000) and Schwartz et al. (2005)

recognize that corporate board decision-making is central to

ethics integration within corporations and decisions of

corporate boards carry expectations of trust, a reciprocal

value to responsibility (Fritzsche 1997). As Ehrlich reminds

us, even many routine questions posed to corporate ethics

officers deal with conflicts of interest that encompass both

the legal and ethical concept of ‘‘duty of loyalty’’ (2005,

p. 4). Thus, values are less ambiguous when perceived as

part of the rules themselves (Hartman 2000) that also reveal

currently perceived ethical demands in today’s business and

regulatory environment (Nesteruk 1999a, b).

Codes that take a coinciding view of legal compliance

and ethics have much more to offer our understanding of

the role of law than those taking a separated view. The

ethical content of law is of such great magnitude that

Nesteruk calls for a new way to view the images of law

(1999a, b). It is so inter-twined with ethics that Salbu

concludes that ethics permeates the law through its

emphasis on equity or fairness, constitutional concepts

such as ‘‘due process’’ and modern statutory law (2001).

His specific examples include the Civil Rights Act of 1964,

cyber law and environmental law (Salbu 2001), each

exemplifying an ‘‘applied ethics’’ (Dunfee 1996, p. 318)

within the substantive law or perhaps a ‘‘substantive eth-

ics.’’ This author therefore suggests that the concept of

‘‘substantive ethics’’ aptly and accurately conveys the

coinciding view of law and business ethics as (1) ethics that

is part of the rights and duties of the substantive law, and

(2) ethics that therefore evolves as the substantive law

evolves. The following employment law example, one area

of ‘‘ethics-permeated’’ laws shown above in Exhibit 4,

illustrates these two aspects of ‘‘substantive ethics.’’

It has been said that education is the answer to guar-

anteeing equal opportunity to all and that the Civil Rights

Acts, and other similar laws are unnecessary. In such a

society, equal employment opportunity would be bestowed

upon the deserving by noble minded, ethical, and moral

people. Unfortunately, much of this ‘‘equal opportunity’’

would be dependent upon the ethical and moral largesse of

many who are the least likely to possess it. Here, as in other

business ethics challenges, substantive law imbued with

ethics values has been the necessary response. It also

reveals the concurrent unfolding of coinciding ethics val-

ues that is analogous to the evolution of the substantive

law. Following is an example as seen in the history of the

common law doctrine of ‘‘employment-at-will.’’

Common law doctrines trace their origin to medieval

England, the source of our legal system that is based upon

stari decisis or the reliance upon case precedent as a guide

to deciding current controversies. The common law system

spans the globe and many of its doctrines are imbued with

ethics, in particular, the ethic of ‘‘individual responsibil-

ity;’’ however, our society has gradually eroded this stark

aspect of the common law by favoring a more societal

viewpoint or sharing of responsibility. According to the

common law doctrine of ‘‘employment at will,’’ either

party can terminate employment at whim without liability

123

46 M. S. Blodgett

(Ballam 2000). This doctrine has been fundamentally

altered over time to produce a more just and responsible

employment environment given the changes that have

evolved with the industrial revolution and our nation’s

commitment to equal opportunity. Among the current

restrictions on this doctrine are ‘‘wrongful discharge’’ and

the protections against some forms of discrimination

offered by the Civil Rights Acts. Evolving societal ethics

have thus transformed this common law doctrine and oth-

ers such as caveat emptor or ‘‘buyer beware.’’

As ethics evolves, so does the legislative response or what

some have called ‘‘legislating ethics’’ (Jennings 2006). This

dynamic of regulatory response to ethical breach is highly

predictable as the demand for business ethics increases.

Hosmer also recognizes this ‘‘complex response’’ where

changing moral standards become institutionalized by social

and political processes (2008, p. 71). Thus, law and ethics

evolve over time as legislation responds to ethical demands.

Perhaps another way of expressing the complexities of

‘‘legislating ethics’’ is through the role of social responsi-

bility, or duty. Just as Ostas recognizes that legal duty is

encompassed by a more expansive social duty, one that

‘‘could be entrusted to the goodwill and ethics of the

business community’’ (2004, p. 563), as a Kantian ‘‘self-

regulator’’ (Herrera 2000, p. 2), he also concludes that if

this duty were not met then regulation would follow. The

Sarbannes-Oxley Act is one of our most recent and dra-

matic examples of this inter-relation of law and ethics—a

breach of business ethics and a legislative response that

demands more ethical behavior.

Other business ethics issues continue to arise, and in

reciprocal fashion, regulations and corporate ethics codes

respond. This should not be surprising because the under-

pinnings of many legal compliance statements such as anti-

trust, securities, employment and intellectual property are

based upon tort law. The business ethics of tort law is

profound. It defines the contours of responsibility, or duty,

and greatly impacts our society’s essential value system

and culture (France 2005). Unfortunately, the ethical and

legal underpinnings and relationships of these compliance

areas are generally not well communicated nor well

understood although ‘‘exemplary conduct usually reflects

an organizational culture and philosophy that is infused

with a sense of responsibility’’ (Paine 1994a, b, p. 109).

Ethical and legal inter-relationships are pervasive, pre-

dictable and continuously evolving. They have an ethical

impact on business and society as people learn about the

law, apply it, and eventually embrace it. Our jurispruden-

tial and legislative processes institutionalize this evolving

ethical impact. As coinciding business ethics evolves, so

does substantive, coinciding law to such an extent that

Hoffman recommends that ethics codes stay abreast of the

latest legal developments (Boatright 2003). The law is not

always a threshold. It is at times an evolving and vast array

of business ethics.

Hence, we arrive at an emerging perspective of law and

business ethics as part of values- or integrity-based ethics

programs—a ‘‘substantive ethics’’ that more descriptively

reflects the evolving and coinciding, inter-relationship of

law and ethics. But how can the image of law be transformed

from the often pervasively narrow view of ‘‘rules to be fol-

lowed’’ to an image that encourages the best applications in

ethical decision-making and corporate governance?

Re-Imaged Corporate Compliance

Emerging perspectives on the relationship between law and

ethics are prompted by Nesteruk’s call for ‘‘re-imagining’’

law (1999a, b) and Paine’s and Trevino’s passionate views

on integrity-based ethics programs where both legal com-

pliance and ethics are recognized as vital, integrative parts

of the whole (1994a, b; Trevino et al. 1999). The inter-

relationship of law and ethics also conforms to Hartman’s

‘‘process of ethics integration’’ (2000, p. 157) where law

and ethics coincide in corporate codes beyond narrow

notions of ‘‘rules to be obeyed’’ and where ethical values

are clarified by law (Hartman 2000). This clarity of values

is consistent with an expanded view of ‘‘shared values,’’

the norms that create organizational expectations (Weaver

and Trevino 1999), as they imbue legal compliance and the

corporation’s total ethical culture. As the values or integ-

rity approach advances, corporate ethics programs may

more distinctively transform beyond a juxtaposition of the

polar realms of compliance and ethical values.

There is now an opportunity for re-imaging the law by

enhancing the compliance parts of corporate codes with

ethics. By re-writing codes to explain and elaborate on the

business ethics context of the law, business can demon-

strate the reasons why laws should be followed and clarify

that although law is not an ethical totality—neither is it

devoid of ethics. Such revised expressions should explain

unambiguously that ethics and law at times coincide, and

they should also make use of identified global values.

For example, the legal compliance statement ‘‘we abide

by securities regulation’’ can be prefaced with the historical

objective of the Securities Acts which is to protect inves-

tors by upholding business ethics through fair dealing (Cox

et al. 2004) or as our courts have stated, by substituting ‘‘a

philosophy of full disclosure for the philosophy of caveat

emptor and thus to achieve a high standard of business

ethics in the securities industry’’ (Di Lorenzo 2007, p. 280).

The author developed an example of such a values or

integrity-based statement set forth in Exhibit 5.

Enhancing compliance statements with their ethical

dimensions will not only communicate that there is ethics

in compliance but enhance the ethical context of business

123

47 Substantive Ethics

Exhibit 5 Integrity-based statement

‘‘Securities regulation’’

Introduction

Our company’s commitment to comply with the securities laws is

consistent with the business ethics objectives of the Securities

Acts. Much of securities law today is concerned with ensuring

‘‘fairness’’ for both consumers and business by balancing the

interests of these two parties. Among the ways it is achieved is

through appropriate transparency and disclosure of material

information. Investors must be adequately informed before they

purchase equities and we must ensure that employees are not

trading on inside-information nor engaging in fraudulent

activities

We believe in treating all of our stakeholders, both internal and

external, in a fair manner. Fairness is highly valued by our

company in any context across all of our business practices,

relationships and commitments

As such, fairness is one of our core and inter-related ethics and

legal compliance values that we use to guide our decision-

making on a daily basis. This integration of ethics and law is part

of our company’s commitment to our shared core values—a

vibrant part of our organizational culture and strength

generally—a perceived ‘‘coming together’’ of ethics and

law that confronts current and anticipated business issues.

The extended context of the value of fairness as indicated

above, is an example of this ethical enhancement and a way

to deepen ethical culture, (Trevino et al. 1998; Paine

1994a, b) invigorate managerial decision-making and cor-

porate governance, and reach for the ‘‘goal to achieving

global and universal standards of fundamental ethical val-

ues’’ (Hoffman and Driscoll 2000, p. 230). Presently, what

may appear as a faint glimmer in corporate ethics codes

that may mention ‘‘integrity’’ and refer to the law and

ethics as ‘‘intertwining concepts,’’ may in reality be a

beacon of change. This enhanced ethics content is consis-

tent with the preferred values or integrity-based ethics

program and as this article asserts, an emerging perspective

on the substantive law and ethics, a ‘‘substantive ethics.’’

Conclusion

This article analyzes coinciding aspects of law and busi-

ness ethics as an emerging perspective in managerial

decision-making and corporate governance. It does so with

the hope of enhancing understanding of the significance of

the law so that it is perceived not as mere rules to be

obeyed, but rather as integral to ethical decision-making.

After all, corporate ethics codes that contain statements of

legal compliance have been identified as a source of

business ethics (Schwartz 2005).

The author reviewed legal compliance statements in

corporate codes of ethics, measured their frequency of

occurrence and explored their ethical content. This article

makes recommendations to revise statements of corporate

compliance to more fully express their ethical content. This

enhanced process of ethics integration is consistent with

the call to transition from legal-compliance-based ethics

programs to values or integrity-based programs rooted in

the idea that the individual internalizes the shared core

values of the firm as a personal commitment to self-gov-

ernance (Trevino et al. 1999; Paine 1994a, b) where law is

well acknowledged and ethics is emphasized.

This recommended approach will help business not only

to reach beyond the legal compliance-based notion that

whatever is legal is the ethical extent of corporate respon-

sibility, yet also help business to see the ethical value in

obeying laws. With this knowledge, business is better able to

apply ethical reasoning to even non-regulated areas as part of

its ethical identity of shared core values and its commitment

to ethical sustainability. Greater compliance as a function of

ethics is an appropriate approach and goal for corporate

governance today where the substantive law is re-imaged as

a vital part of business ethics, a ‘‘substantive ethics.’’

References

3M Compliance Policy. Retrieved January 1, 2009, from http://solutions.3m.

com/wps/portal/3M/en_US/businessconduct/bcmain/policy/policies/

legalethic/compliance/.

Altria, Responsibility—Compliance & Integrity at Altria Group.

Retrieved December 5, 2009, from http://altria.com/responsibility/

4_//_3_respci.asp.

August, R., Mayer, D., & Bixby, M. (2009). International business
law (pp. 8–9).

Upper

Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Avery Dennison, Code of Ethics and Business Conduct. Retrieved

December 5, 2009, from http://www.averydennison.com/corporate.

nsf/1ebb2cd9540ffe2a882569de00829ce0/67b66.

Ballam, D. (2000). Employment-at-will: The impending death of a

doctrine. American Business Law Journal, 37(4), 653–687.

Blodgett, M., & Carlson, P. (1997). Corporate ethics codes: A

practical application of liability prevention. Journal of Business
Ethics, 16(12/13), 1363–1369.

Boatright, J. (2003). Ethics and the conduct of business (4th ed.).

Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Cavanaugh, G. F. (1998). American business values (4th ed.). Upper

Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Cohen, D. (1993). Creating and maintaining ethical work climates:

Anomie in the workplace and implications for managing change.

Business Ethics Quarterly, 3(4), 343–358.

Cox, J., Hillman, R., & Langevoort, D. (2004). Securities regulation
(4th ed.). New York, NY: Aspen Publishers.

Di Lorenzo, V. (2007). Business ethics: Law as a determinant of

business conduct. Journal of Business Ethics, 7(3), 275–299.

Donaldson, T., & Dunfee, T. (1999). Ties that bind: A social contracts
approach to business ethics. Boston, MA: Harvard Business

School Press.

Donaldson, T., & Werhane, P. (1993). Ethical issues in business.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Dunfee, T. (1996). On the synergistic, interdependent relationship of

business ethics and law. American Business Law Journal, 34(2),

316–319.

123

http://solutions.3m.com/wps/portal/3M/en_US/businessconduct/bcmain/policy/policies/legalethic/compliance/

http://solutions.3m.com/wps/portal/3M/en_US/businessconduct/bcmain/policy/policies/legalethic/compliance/

http://solutions.3m.com/wps/portal/3M/en_US/businessconduct/bcmain/policy/policies/legalethic/compliance/

http://altria.com/responsibility/4_//_3_respci.asp

http://altria.com/responsibility/4_//_3_respci.asp

http://www.averydennison.com/corporate.nsf/1ebb2cd9540ffe2a882569de00829ce0/67b66

http://www.averydennison.com/corporate.nsf/1ebb2cd9540ffe2a882569de00829ce0/67b66

48 M. S. Blodgett

Ehrlich, C. (2005). Is business ethics necessary? DePaul Business and
Commercial Law Journal, 4, 55.

France, M. (2005). How to fix the tort system. Business Week, 14,

70–78.

Fritzsche, D. (1997). Business ethics: A global and managerial
perspective. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Fuller, L. (1958). Positivism and fidelity to law—a reply to professor

Hart. Harvard Law Review, 71(4), 630–672.

Hart, H. L. A. (1958). Positivism and the separation of law and

morals. Harvard Law Review, 71(4), 593–629.

Hartman, L. (2000). Compliance versus integrity: The process of

ethics integration. Journal of Employment Discrimination Law,
2(2), 157–159.

Herrera, C. D. (2000). How are Law and Ethics Related? Retrieved

April 30, 2009, from http://www-hsc.usc.edu/*mbernste/tae.ethics

&law.herrera.html.

Hilts, P. (2003). Protecting America’s health (pp. 45–46). Chapel

Hill, NC: The University of North Carolina Press.

Hoffman, W. M., & Driscoll, D. M. (2000). Business ethics in the new

millennium: Will the patient survive? Business Ethics Quarterly,
10(1), 221–231.

Hosmer, L. (2008). The ethics of management (6th ed., p. 71). New

York, NY: McGraw Hill Irwin.

Jennings, M. (2006). Business ethics (p. 3). Mason, OH: Thomson-

West.

Murphy, P. (2005). Developing, communicating and promoting

corporate ethics statements: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of
Business Ethics, 62(2), 183–189.

Nesteruk, J. (1999a). Reimagining the law. Business Ethics Quarterly,
9(4), 603–617.

Nesteruk, J. (1999b). Evaluating the moral creativity of the law.

Business Ethics Quarterly, 9(4), 689–692.

Ostas, D. (2004). Cooperate, comply or evade? A corporate execu-

tive’s social responsibilities with regard to law. American
Business Law Journal, 41(4), 559–594.

Pacific life, our principals, our values: A guide to the code of business

conduct. Retrieved December 5, 2009, from http://www.pacific

life.com/About?Pacific?Life/Corporate?Governance/Corporate?

Governance2.htm.

Pagnatarro, M. A., & Peirce, E. (2007). Between a rock and a hard

place; the conflict between U.S. corporate codes of conduct and

European privacy and work laws. Berkley Journal of Employ-
ment and Labor Law, 28, 375–428.

Paine, L. S. (1994a). Law, ethics, & managerial judgment. Journal of
Legal Studies Education, 12(2), 153–169.

Paine, L. S. (1994b). Managing for organizational integrity. Harvard
Business Review, 72(2), 106–117.

Peppas, S. (2002). Attitudes toward business ethics: Where east

doesn’t meet west. Cross Cultural Management, 9, 42–59.

Post, J., Lawrence, A., & Weber, J. (2002). ‘Ethical dilemmas in
business’, business and society (10th ed.). New York, NY:
McGraw-Hill.

Reed, O. L., Shedd, P., Morehead, J., & Pagnattaro, M. (2009). The
legal and regulatory environment of business (14th ed.).

NewYork, NY: Irwin/McGrawHill.

Salbu, S. (2001). Law and ethics. American Business Law Journal,
38(2), 209–214.

Schaffer, R., Agusti, F., & Earle, B. (2009). International business
law and its environment (7th ed.). Mason, OH: South-Western

Cengage Learning.

Schwartz, M. (2005). Universal moral values for corporate codes of

ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 59(1/2), 27–44.

Schwartz, M., Dunfee, T., & Kline, M. (2005). Tone at the top: An

ethics code for directors? Journal of Business Ethics, 58(1–3),

79–100.

Shaw, W. H. (1996). Business ethics (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA:

Wadsworth Publishing Co.

Trevino, L., Butterfield, K., & McCabe, D. (1998). The ethical

context in organizations: Influences on employee attitudes and

behaviors. Business Ethics Quarterly, 8(3), 447–476.

Trevino, L., Weaver, G., Gibson, D., & Toffler, B. (1999). Managing

ethics and legal compliance: What works and what hurts.

California Management Review, 41(2), 131–151.

Weaver, G., & Trevino, L. (1999). Compliance and values oriented

ethics programs: Influences on employee’s attitudes and behav-

ior. Business Ethics Quarterly, 9(2), 315–335.

Webley, S. (1997). The business organisation. In P. W. F. Davies

(Ed.), Current issues in business ethics. London: Routledge.

123

http://www-hsc.usc.edu/~mbernste/tae.ethics&law.herrera.html

http://www-hsc.usc.edu/~mbernste/tae.ethics&law.herrera.html

http://www.pacificlife.com/About%2bPacific%2bLife/Corporate%2bGovernance/Corporate%2bGovernance2.htm

http://www.pacificlife.com/About%2bPacific%2bLife/Corporate%2bGovernance/Corporate%2bGovernance2.htm

http://www.pacificlife.com/About%2bPacific%2bLife/Corporate%2bGovernance/Corporate%2bGovernance2.htm

Copyright of Journal of Business Ethics is the property of Springer Science & Business Media
B.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder’s express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

Journal of Applied Psychology
2001, Vol. 86, No. 3, 425-445

Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0021-9010/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0021-9010.86.3.425

Justice at the Millennium: A Meta-Analytic Review of 25 Years of
Organizational Justice Research

Jason A. Colquitt
University of Florida

Michael J. Wesson and Christopher O. L. H. Porter
Texas A&M University

Donald E. Conlon
Michigan State University

K. Yee Ng
Michigan State University

The field of organizational justice continues to be marked by several important research questions,
including the size of relationships among justice dimensions, the relative importance of different justice
criteria, and the unique effects of justice dimensions on key outcomes. To address such questions, the
authors conducted a meta-analytic review of 183 justice studies. The results suggest that although
different justice dimensions are moderately to highly related, they contribute incremental variance
explained in fairness perceptions. The results also illustrate the overall and unique relationships among
distributive, procedural, interpersonal, and informational justice and several organizational outcomes
(e.g., job satisfaction, organizational commitment, evaluation of authority, organizational citizenship
behavior, withdrawal, performance). These findings are reviewed in terms of their implications for future
research on organizational justice.

The study of justice or fairness has been a topic of philosophical
interest that extends back at least as far as Plato and Socrates
(Ryan, 1993). Colloquially, the term justice is used to connote
“oughtness” or “righteousness.” Under the purview of ethics, an
act can be defined as just through comparison with a prevailing
philosophical system. Unfortunately, often there is no agreement
on what that philosophical system should be. For example, Aris­
totle (reprinted in Frost, 1972) noted that people in different roles
will advocate different justice rules, arguing that “the democrats
are for freedom, oligarchs for wealth, others for nobleness of birth”
(p. 136; see also Pillutla & Mumighan, 1999).

In research in the organizational sciences, justice is considered
to be socially constructed. That is, an act is defined as just if most
individuals perceive it to be so on the basis of empirical research
(Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997). Thus, “what is fair” is derived
from past research linking objective facets of decision making to
subjective perceptions of fairness. In particular, justice in organi­
zational settings can be described as focusing on the antecedents
and consequences of two types of subjective perceptions: (a) the
fairness of outcome distributions or allocations and (b) the fairness
of the procedures used to determine outcome distributions or
allocations. These forms of justice are typically referred to as

Jason A. Colquitt, Department of Management, University of Florida;
Donald E. Conlon and K. Yee Ng, Department of Management, Michigan
State University; Michael J. Wesson and Christopher O. L. H. Porter,
Department of Management, Texas A&M University.

We thank George Barnekov and Yumi Eto for their assistance in
gathering the articles covered in this review.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jason A.
Colquitt, Warrington College of Business Administration, Department of
Management, University of Florida, P.O. Box 117165, Gainesville, Florida
32611-7165. Electronic mail may be sent to colquitt@ufl.edu.

distributive justice (Adams, 1965; Deutsch, 1975; Homans, 1961;
Leventhal, 1976) and procedural justice (Leventhal, 1980; Lev­
enthal, Karuža, & Fry, 1980; Thibaut & Walker, 1975).

Efforts to explain the impact of justice on effective organiza­
tional functioning have come under the rubric of organizational
justice research (Greenberg, 1987b, 1990b). Greenberg (1990b)
described organizational justice as a literature “grown around
attempts to describe and explain the role of fairness as a consid­
eration in the workplace” (p. 400). This literature includes both
field and laboratory research, and organizational justice has been
among the most frequently researched topics in industrial-
organizational psychology, human resource management, and or­
ganizational behavior over the last decade (Cropanzano & Green­
berg, 1997).

As interest in organizational justice has proliferated, so too have
the theoretical approaches used to study it, particularly in relation
to procedural justice. These approaches each propose a different
way of conceptualizing justice, from the provision of process
control (Thibaut & Walker, 1975) to a focus on consistency
(Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal et al., 1980) and an examination of
interpersonal treatment (Bies & Moag, 1986). In addition, a large
number of studies have sought to link justice perceptions to a
variety of organizational outcomes, including job satisfaction, or­
ganizational commitment, withdrawal, and organizational citizen­
ship behavior.

Because of this diversity in theoretical approach and construct
focus, organizational justice is a field in need of integration. There
have been a number of narrative reviews that have sought to
achieve such integration (e.g., Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997;
Folger & Cropanzano, 1998; Lind & Tyler, 1988; Greenberg,
1987b, 1990b). However, important questions remain, including
the following: How highly related are the different dimensions of
organizational justice, and can they be empirically distinguished

425

mailto:colquitt@ufl.edu

COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG426

from one another? Have the different ways of conceptualizing
justice improved our ability to create perceptions of fairness? and
What are the relationships between different organizational justice
dimensions and important outcomes relevant to organizations? The
first question deals with issues of construct discrimination (i.e., to
what extent are constructs distinct from one another?). The second
question deals with what Greenberg (1987b) called proactive re­
search (i.e., research devoted to creating perceptions of fairness).
The last question deals with what Greenberg (1987b) called reac­
tive research (i.e., research devoted to understanding how individ­
uals react to fair or unfair treatment).

The purpose of this study was to conduct a comprehensive,
meta-analytic review of the existing literature on organizational
justice. To our knowledge, a meta-analysis has never been con­
ducted in the organizational justice literature, even though meta­
analysis has several important strengths relative to traditional
narrative reviews (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). First, meta-analysis
is a more powerful summarizing tool because it yields a quantita­
tive population value for a relationship of interest. Second, meta­
analysis typically includes more studies than narrative reviews and
is therefore less susceptible to biases based on study inclusion.
Third, meta-analysis allows the reviewer to understand why rela­
tionships in the literature vary as a function of sampling error,
measurement error, and moderator variables. These strengths make
meta-analysis an effective way of examining the three types of
questions just listed.

We conducted meta-analyses on all articles in the organizational
justice literature published since 1975. We chose 1975 as our
starting date because this is when Thibaut and Walker introduced
the procedural justice construct, which allowed for the compara­
tive study of the influence of multiple dimensions of justice. That
year also marks, approximately, the time when justice researchers
began integrating fairness concerns with outcomes relevant to
organizations (e.g., job satisfaction and organizational commit­
ment). Thus, as we reach the millennium, this review covers a
quarter century of academic research. Our analyses dealt with all
three types of questions raised earlier: construct discrimination
issues, proactive research issues, and reactive research issues. In
total, we relied on 120 separate meta-analyses, along with the
emerging technique of meta-analytic regression analysis (Viswes-
varan & Ones, 1995), to explore the three types of questions. We
provide a brief review of the organizational justice literature before
reviewing the specific research questions explored in this article.

A Brief Review of the Organizational Justice Literature

Introduction of Distributive Justice

Before 1975, the study of justice was primarily concerned with
distributive justice. Much of this research was derived from initial
work conducted by Adams (1965), who used a social exchange
theory framework to evaluate fairness. According to Adams, what
people were concerned about was not the absolute level of out­
comes per se but whether those outcomes were fair. Adams sug­
gested that one way to determine whether an outcome was fair was
to calculate the ratio of one’s contributions or “inputs” (e.g.,
education, intelligence, and experience) to one’s outcome and then
compare that ratio with that of a comparison other. Although the
comparison of the two input-outcome ratios gives Adams’s equity

theory an “objective” component, he was clear that this process
was completely subjective.

Whereas Adams’s theory advocated the use of an equity rule to
determine fairness, several other allocation rules have also been
identified, such as equality and need (e.g., Leventhal, 1976).
Studies have shown that different contexts (e.g., work vs. family),
different organizational goals (e.g., group harmony vs. productiv­
ity), and different personal motives (e.g., self-interest motives vs.
altruistic motives) can activate the use or primacy of certain
allocation rules (Deutsch, 1975). Nevertheless, all of the allocation
standards have as their goal the achievement of distributive justice;
they merely attempt to create it through the use of different rules.

Introduction of Procedural Justice

With the publication of their book summarizing disputant reac­
tions to legal procedures, Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced
the study of process to the literature on justice. Thibaut and Walker
(1975) viewed third-party dispute resolution procedures such as
mediation and arbitration as having both a process stage and a
decision stage. They referred to the amount of influence disputants
had in each stage as evidence of process control and decision
control, respectively. Their research suggested that disputants were
willing to give up control in the decision stage as long as they
retained control in the process stage. Stated differently, disputants
viewed the procedure as fair if they perceived that they had process
control (i.e., control over the presentation of their arguments and
sufficient time to present their case). This process control effect is
often referred to as the “fair process effect” or “voice” effect (e.g.,
Folger, 1977; Lind & Tyler, 1988), and it is one of the most
replicated findings in the justice literature. Indeed, Thibaut and
Walker (1975) virtually equated process control with procedural
justice (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998).

Although Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the concept of
procedural justice, their work focused primarily on disputant re­
actions to legal procedures. Although a focus on justice and law
continues to be of interest to scholars (e.g., Tyler, 1990), Leventhal
and colleagues can be credited for extending the notion of proce­
dural justice into nonlegal contexts such as organizational settings
(Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal et al., 1980). In doing so, Leventhal
and colleagues also broadened the list of determinants of proce­
dural justice far beyond the concept of process control. Leventhal’s
theory of procedural justice judgments focused on six criteria that
a procedure should meet if it is to be perceived as fair. Procedures
should (a) be applied consistently across people and across time,
(b) be free from bias (e.g., ensuring that a third party has no vested
interest in a particular settlement), (c) ensure that accurate infor­
mation is collected and used in making decisions, (d) have some
mechanism to correct flawed or inaccurate decisions, (e) conform
to personal or prevailing standards of ethics or morality, and (f)
ensure that the opinions of various groups affected by the decision
have been taken into account.

Introduction of Interactional Justice

Bies and Moag (1986) introduced the most recent advance in the
justice literature by focusing attention on the importance of the
quality of the interpersonal treatment people receive when proce­
dures are implemented. Bies and Moag (1986) referred to these

META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 427

aspects of justice as “interactional justice.” More recently, inter­
actional justice has come to be seen as consisting of two specific
types of interpersonal treatment (e.g., Greenberg, 1990a, 1993b).
The first, labeled interpersonal justice, reflects the degree to which
people are treated with politeness, dignity, and respect by author­
ities or third parties involved in executing procedures or determin­
ing outcomes. The second, labeled informational justice, focuses
on the explanations provided to people that convey information
about why procedures were used in a certain way or why outcomes
were distributed in a certain fashion.

Questions Permeating the Organizational
Justice Literature

The proliferation of studies on organizational justice has cer­
tainly enhanced the visibility of fairness concerns, but the large
number of studies and the differing theoretical perspectives raise
the concern that justice scholars may be “losing the forest for the
trees.” In other words, despite the large accumulation of findings,
many central questions remain either unaddressed or unclear. Until
such questions are addressed, theory development in the organi­
zational justice literature will continue to be hindered. Our meta-
analytic review examines some of these central questions, with
specific areas of focus discussed in the sections to follow.

Construct Discrimination Questions

Perhaps the oldest debate in the justice literature concerns the
independence of procedural and distributive justice. Some studies
have revealed extremely high correlations between the two justice
dimensions, suggesting that they may not be distinct in the minds
of many people (Folger, 1987). For example, Sweeney and Mc­
Farlin (1997) found an uncorrected correlation of .72 between
procedural and distributive justice in a study of attitudes among
federal employees. Welbourne, Balkin, and Gomez-Mejia (1995)
found an uncorrected correlation of .74 in a study of employees in
two different companies, one a high-technology firm and the other
a consumer products firm. Possibly as a result of such findings,
Martocchio and Judge (1995) conducted a study of disciplinary
decisions in which no effort was made to separate procedural and
distributive justice. Rather, the authors examined the effects of
“organizational justice.”

Such high correlations are congruent with theoretical arguments
made by Cropanzano and Ambrose (2001). These authors have
argued that the procedural justice-distributive justice distinction,
although necessary and valuable, may sometimes be overempha­
sized. Their “monistic perspective” notes that procedural evalua­
tions are based in large part on outcomes attained (Thibaut &
Walker, 1975) and that the same event can be seen as a process in
one context and an outcome in another. For example, reorganizing
a performance evaluation system so that it provides employees
more process control can be termed a fair outcome, even though
process control is a procedural construct.

The construct discrimination concern applies to an even greater
degree to procedural and interactional justice. Bies and Moag
(1986) originally declared interactional justice to be a third type of
justice. They argued that people draw on interactional justice
perceptions when deciding how to react to authority figures (i.e.,
bosses and supervisors), whereas procedural justice perceptions

are used to decide how to react to the overall organization. How­
ever, Bies retracted the position that interactional justice was a
third type of justice in a subsequent review (Tyler & Bies, 1990).
The author’s retraction of his earlier stance has become widely
held, as one recent narrative review treated interactional justice as
a social form of procedural justice (Cropanzano & Greenberg,
1997). In keeping with this view, many researchers have opera­
tionalized procedural justice by measuring process control or Lev­
enthal criteria, along with interactional justice, in one combined
scale (e.g., Brockner, Siegel, Daly, & Martin, 1997; Brockner,
Wiesenfeld, & Martin, 1995; Folger & Konovsky, 1989;
Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki
& Latham, 1997). This practice seems to suggest that the inter­
personal implementation of procedures need not (or cannot) be
separated from their structural aspects.

However, other research has renewed the debate surrounding the
distinctiveness of procedural and interactional justice. Studies that
have examined the two constructs separately have shown that they
have different correlates or independent effects, or both (e.g.,
Barling & Phillips, 1993; Blader & Tyler, 2000; Cropanzano &
Prehar, 1999; Masterson, Lewis, Goldman, & Taylor, 2000; Moye,
Masterson, & Bartol, 1997; Skarlicki & Folger, 1997). Blader and
Tyler (2000), in a survey of 404 U.S. workers, found that system­
originating procedural factors and leader-originating procedural
factors remained distinct in a confirmatory factor analysis. Master­
son, Lewis, et al. (2000) drew on social exchange theory to show
that procedural and interactional justice affected other variables
through different intervening mechanisms. Specifically, proce­
dural justice affected other variables by altering perceived organi­
zational support perceptions; interactional justice affected other
variables by altering leader-member exchange perceptions (Graen
& Scandura, 1987).

Even assuming that interactional justice can and should be
distinguished from procedural justice, another question is whether
the interpersonal and informational facets of the construct merit
conceptual separation. Greenberg (1993b) suggested that interper­
sonal and informational justice should be separated because they
are logically distinct and have been shown to have independent
effects (e.g., Greenberg, 1993c, 1994). Interpersonal justice acts
primarily to alter reactions to decision outcomes, because sensi­
tivity can make people feel better about an unfavorable outcome.
Informational justice acts primarily to alter reactions to proce­
dures, in that explanations provide the information needed to
evaluate structural aspects of the process.

Some recent work has attempted to address the construct dis­
crimination questions raised earlier. Colquitt (2001) developed
measures of distributive, procedural, informational, and interper­
sonal justice based on the seminal introductions of each construct
(Bies & Moag, 1986; Leventhal, 1976, 1980; Leventhal et al.,
1980; Thibaut & Walker, 1975) and validated them in both a
university and a field setting. Colquitt found that a four-factor
confirmatory model provided the best fit to the data and further
showed that the four justice dimensions predicted different out­
comes. Thus, although some progress is being made, the literature
on organizational justice is still marked by a debate over whether
the domain includes one, two, three, or four dimensions of justice.
This is the subject of the first research question addressed in our
meta-analytic review.

428 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG

Research Question 1: How highly related are the different dimensions
of organizational justice, and can they be empirically distinguished
from one another?

Proactive Research Questions

The history of research devoted to understanding what promotes
perceptions of fairness has been marked by increasing complexity
in operationalization as new and different conceptualizations have
been introduced over the years. Thibaut and Walker (1975) ini­
tially assumed that perceptions of procedural fairness were driven
by process control. Leventhal and colleagues (Leventhal, 1980;
Leventhal et al., 1980), on the other hand, eschewed the parsimo­
nious approach offered by Thibaut and Walker and instead argued
that fairness perceptions were created by adherence to six different
criteria. Bies and Moag (1986) further suggested that fairness
perceptions were created by the proper enactment of procedures in
terms of interpersonal and informational justice.

Such additional complexity is warranted if new approaches
contribute incrementally to our understanding of how to foster
perceptions of procedural fairness. Unfortunately, at present we do
not actually know whether such incremental contributions exist.
This is because so many studies of procedural justice collapse
process control, Leventhal criteria, and interpersonal and informa­
tional justice into a single variable (e.g., Brockner et al., 1995,
1997; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Folger, 1991;
Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). This
makes it impossible to gauge the relative influence of each element
on procedural fairness perceptions. Still other studies assess only
one conceptualization of procedural justice, again making it im­
possible to assess the merits of different approaches.

Moreover, it is well accepted that people judge procedures as
more fair when they result in fair or favorable outcomes (Lind &
Tyler, 1988; Thibaut & Walker, 1975). Despite this, many studies
of procedural justice do not also examine distributive justice. Other
studies do examine distributive justice but use it only in analyses
separate from procedural justice. As a result, we cannot even be
sure that process control, Leventhal criteria, and so forth explain
variance in procedural fairness perceptions beyond distributive
justice. If the operationalizations of procedural justice contribute
little to procedural fairness perceptions beyond simple outcome
fairness, then the focus on additional conceptualizations has been
unwarranted.

Thus, in terms of creating procedural fairness perceptions, it is
unclear to what extent new conceptualizations of procedural jus­
tice (including the relatively recent introductions of interpersonal
and informational justice) have contributed to our understanding.
Nor is it clear how important they are beyond the fairness of
outcome distributions. This is the subject of our next two research
questions.

Research Question 2: Have the additional conceptualizations of pro­
cedural justice developed over time (including interpersonal and in­
formational justice) contributed incremental variance explained in
procedural fairness perceptions?

Research Question 3: Do those conceptualizations contribute incre­
mental variance explained in procedural fairness perceptions beyond
the effects of distributive justice?

Reactive Research Questions

Of course, one of the reasons scholars study justice is the belief
that enhanced fairness perceptions can improve outcomes relevant
to organizations (e.g., organizational commitment, job satisfaction,
and performance). One goal of our meta-analytic review is to help
develop some consensus regarding the relationships between di­
mensions of justice and key outcomes. In particular, we evaluate
the predictive ability of three different models that attempt to
explain relationships between justice dimensions and important
outcomes. These three models are the distributive dominance
model suggested by Leventhal (1980), the two-factor model sug­
gested by Sweeney and McFarlin (1993), and the agent-system
model suggested by Bies and Moag (1986).

The relative predictive power of procedural and distributive
justice has been of long-standing concern in the justice literature.
An interesting aspect of Leventhal’s (1980) work is that he ex­
plicitly considered the impact of both procedural and distributive
justice and argued that distributive justice is generally more salient
than procedural justice. He further argued that distributive justice
judgments are likely to be more influential than procedural justice
judgments in determining “overall fairness judgments” (Leventhal,
1980, p. 133; Lind & Tyler, 1988). Consistent with this argument,
Conlon (1993) found that distributive justice explained more vari­
ance in grievant evaluations of authorities (an appeal board) than
did procedural justice. These works support a strong, parsimonious
prediction that distributive justice will dominate (i.e., explain more
variance than) other forms of justice.

However, some prior research and theory have questioned this
thesis. In perhaps the first empirical study to explicitly link mul­
tiple dimensions of justice to organizational outcomes, Alexander
and Ruderman (1987) surveyed more than 2,000 federal employ­
ees, measuring procedural and distributive justice along with six
outcomes. Their regression analyses showed that justice measures
affected five of six organizational outcomes and that, for four of
these five outcomes, procedural justice had stronger relationships
than distributive justice.

More recently, scholars have argued that distributive justice is
likely to exert greater influence on more specific, person-
referenced outcomes such as satisfaction with a pay raise or
performance evaluation. In contrast, procedural justice is likely to
exert greater influence on more general evaluations of systems and
authorities (Greenberg, 1990b; Lind & Tyler, 1988). Consistent
with this prediction, McFarlin and Sweeney (1992) found that
distributive justice was a more important predictor of what they
termed two “personal outcomes” (pay satisfaction and job satis­
faction) and that procedural justice was a more important predictor
of two “organizational outcomes” (organizational commitment and
subordinate’s evaluation of supervisor).

Sweeney and McFarlin (1993) conducted perhaps the most
comprehensive test of this notion. They contrasted four models
expressing the relationship between procedural and distributive
justice and two outcomes (pay satisfaction and organizational
commitment). The idea that procedural justice predicts more
system-referenced outcomes and distributive justice predicts more
person-referenced outcomes was termed in their article the “two-
factor model.” They found that the two-factor model fit their data
better than three competing models. Thus, drawing on the results

META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 429

of these studies, we examine the two-factor model’s prediction of
differing effects for procedural and distributive justice.

Finally, Bies and Moag’s (1986) focus on interpersonal treat­
ment suggests a third approach to explaining organizational out­
comes, one that explicitly examines the role of interpersonal and
informational justice in addition to procedural justice. Recall that
Bies and Moag (1986) originally argued that individuals draw on
interpersonal and informational justice perceptions when deciding
how to react to authority figures (i.e., bosses and supervisors) and
draw on procedural justice perceptions when deciding how to react
to the overall organization. Building on this work, Masterson,
Lewis, et al. (2000) drew on social exchange theory (Blau, 1964)
and reasoned that individuals in organizations were involved in
two types of exchange relationships: exchanges with their imme­
diate supervisor and exchanges with the larger organization. In a
field study, the authors showed that interactional justice predicted
supervisor-referenced outcomes (e.g., citizenship behaviors di­
rected at supervisor and supervisor rating of performance),
whereas procedural justice predicted organization-referenced out­
comes (e.g., citizenship behaviors directed at the organization and
organizational commitment). Thus, our third reactive model, the
agent-system model, reflects the assertion that interpersonal and
informational justice will be more powerful predictors of agent-
referenced outcomes than system-referenced outcomes.

The distributive dominance model, the two-factor model, and
the agent-system model are explored in the following set of
research questions.

Research Question 4: What are the relationships between distributive
justice and important organizational outcomes?

Research Question 5: What are the relationships between procedural
justice and important organizational outcomes?

Research Question 6: What are the relationships between interper­
sonal and informational justice and important organizational
outcomes?

Research Question 7: If considered simultaneously, what are the
unique effects of these justice dimensions on key outcomes, and
which of the three reactive models receives the most support?

Our review focuses on several different outcomes representing
those most commonly examined in the organizational justice lit­
erature. In the following sections, we briefly review each type of
outcome.

Outcome satisfaction. Many justice studies have measured
satisfaction with the outcomes of a decision-making process, such
as pay, promotions, and performance evaluations. Given the logic
presented earlier, we expect that distributive justice judgments will
be a better predictor of outcome satisfaction than will procedural
justice or interpersonal and informational justice. This pattern has
been empirically supported through the use of pay satisfaction and
satisfaction with job restructuring (e.g., Folger & Konovsky, 1989;
Lowe & Vodanovich, 1995; Sweeney & McFarlin, 1993), and it is
consistent with both the distributive dominance and two-factor
models.

Job satisfaction. Many studies also ask about employees’ sat­
isfaction with their jobs in general. McFarlin and Sweeney (1992)
showed that distributive justice was a more powerful predictor of
job satisfaction than was procedural justice. However, this does
not seem to fit the two-factor theory argument that procedural

justice predicts system-referenced outcomes, whereas distributive
justice predicts person-referenced outcomes. Job satisfaction is a
more general, multifaceted, and global response than is outcome
satisfaction. Consistent with this reasoning, other studies have
shown high correlations between procedural justice and job satis­
faction (e.g., Mossholder, Bennett, & Martin, 1998; Wesolowski &
Mossholder, 1997). In addition, Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000)
showed procedural justice to be a stronger predictor of job satis­
faction than interactional justice, although both had significant
independent effects. These results are consistent with the two-
factor model and the agent-system model.

Organizational commitment. Organizational commitment rep­
resents a global, systemic reaction that people have to the company
for which they work. Most measures of organizational commit­
ment assess affective commitment, the degree to which employees
identify with the company and make the company’s goals their
own (Allen & Meyer, 1990). Prior work by Tyler (e.g., Tyler,
1990) argues that procedural justice has stronger relationships with
support for institutions than does distributive justice. This is also
consistent with the two-factor model and has been supported in
several studies (e.g., Folger & Konovsky, 1989; McFarlin &
Sweeney, 1992; Sweeney & McFarlin, 1993). However, we should
note that several studies have instead supported the distributive
dominance model. For example, Lowe and Vodanovich (1995)
found a stronger relationship for distributive justice and organiza­
tional commitment than for procedural justice, as did Greenberg
(1994). Other results support the agent-system model, in which
procedural justice is a stronger predictor of organizational com­
mitment than interactional justice (Masterson, Lewis, et al., 2000).

Trust. Trust has recently emerged as a popular topic in orga­
nizational research (as evidenced by the 1998 Academy of Man­
agement Review special issue devoted to the topic). Tyler (1989)
argued that trust in decision makers or authorities is important
because these people typically have considerable discretion in
terms of allocating rewards and resources. Whereas Tyler (1989)
initially conceptualized trust in relation to a third party or an
authority, Folger and Cropanzano (1998) made the point that trust
reactions are relevant to any person with whom one is interdepen­
dent. Given the centrality of trust in theorizing on procedural
justice, we would expect to find stronger relationships between
trust and procedural justice than between trust and distributive
justice, consistent with past research (e.g., Alexander & Ruder­
man, 1987; Konovsky & Pugh, 1994). However, given that trust is
usually referenced to a particular person, the agent-system model
would predict that interpersonal and informational justice are even
better predictors of this outcome than procedural justice.

Evaluation of authority. A number of studies of third-party
dispute resolution procedures have asked disputants to make eval­
uations of the third party (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Still other work in
organizations asks respondents to rate the acceptability of their
supervisors or management in more general terms. Much of the
research on evaluation of authorities comes from work merging
psychology and political science (e.g., Tyler, 1990). Tyler’s (1990)
work, along with the two-factor model, would suggest that we
should find stronger relationships between procedural justice and
evaluation of authorities than between distributive justice and
evaluation of authorities. However, as with organizational com­
mitment, this prediction has been supported in multiple studies
(e.g., Ball, Trevino, & Sims, 1993; McFarlin & Sweeney, 1992)

430 COLQUITT. CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG

and refuted in multiple studies (e.g., Conlon, 1993; Taylor, Tracy,
Renard, Harrison, & Carroll, 1995). In addition, the agent-system
model would predict that interpersonal and informational justice
are better predictors of evaluation of authority in cases in which
the authority in question is one’s leader as opposed to management
in general. For this reason, our examination of the reactive models
distinguishes between agent-referenced evaluations of authority
(e.g., focusing on one’s supervisor) and system-referenced evalu­
ations of authority (e.g., focusing on management in general).

Organizational citizenship behaviors (OCBs). Organ (1990)
defined OCBs as behaviors that are discretionary and not explicitly
rewarded but that can help improve organizational functioning.
Organ (1990) posited that OCBs are driven largely by fairness
perceptions. He suggested that people in organizations assume, at
the outset, a social exchange relationship. This expectation con­
tinues until unfairness is evidenced, at which time the relationship
is reinterpreted as economic rather than social. Research on OCBs
has repeatedly demonstrated stronger linkages between procedural
justice and OCBs than between distributive justice and OCBs
(Ball, Trevino, & Sims, 1994; Moorman, 1991). For example,
Moorman (1991) reported that procedural justice influenced four
of five OCB dimensions, whereas distributive justice failed to
influence any dimensions. Skarlicki and Latham (1996) even
showed that training supervisors on procedural justice principles
was capable of improving OCB levels. To the extent that OCBs
were measured in relation to supervisors rather than the organiza­
tion as a whole, we would expect interpersonal and informational
justice to be stronger predictors, consistent with the agent-system
model and the results of Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000). Thus, our
examination of the reactive models distinguishes between agent-
referenced OCBs and system-referenced OCBs. Following Wil­
liams and Anderson (1991), we refer to the former as individual
OCBs (OCBIs) and the latter as organization OCBs (OCBOs).

Withdrawal. Behaviors and behavioral intentions such as ab­
senteeism, turnover, and neglect are often subsumed under the
heading of job withdrawal. Although withdrawal is a relatively
common outcome in the justice literature, it has not been examined
in the context of the two-factor model. Withdrawal can occur as a
result of a thorough, reasoned evaluation of the organization as a
system or on a more “spur of the moment” basis in reaction to an
unsatisfactory outcome or poor interpersonal treatment by an au­
thority. However, because employees who withdraw are typically
leaving the overall organization, we would argue that withdrawal
is system referenced in nature, similar to organizational commit­
ment. Unfortunately, the literature linking different justice dimen­
sions to withdrawal is somewhat muddied, with some studies
showing that distributive justice influences job withdrawal (e.g.,
Hom, Griffeth, & Sellare, 1984) and other studies revealing effects
for procedural justice (e.g., Dailey & Kirk, 1992). Moreover,
Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000) showed that procedural justice had
more of an impact on withdrawal than interactional justice. Thus,
past research has, at various times, supported the distributive
dominance model, the two-factor model, and the agent-system
model.

Negative reactions. Some recent justice research has looked at
the relationship between perceived unfairness and a variety of
negative reactions, such as employee theft (e.g., Greenberg, 1990a,
1993c) and organizational retaliatory behaviors (ORBs; Skarlicki
& Folger, 1997; Skarlicki, Folger, & Tesluk, 1999). As with

withdrawal, negative reactions have not been examined from the
standpoint of the two-factor model. Whereas negative reactions
can occur because of purely cognitive evaluations of the merits of
the organization as a whole or as strong emotional reactions to
one’s own treatment, reactions such as theft and ORBs clearly
damage the larger organizational system. However, Skarlicki and
Folger (1997) found that ORBs had approximately equal correla­
tions with distributive, procedural, and interactional justice, with
interactional justice having the strongest unique effect. To the
extent that ORBs are system-referenced outcomes, this provides
little support for any of the three reactive models.

Performance. Perhaps the most unclear of all relationships in
the justice literature is the relationship between procedural justice
and performance. For example, Earley and Lind (1987) found a
relationship between procedural fairness judgments and perfor­
mance in a laboratory study but not in a field study. Kanfer,
Sawyer, Earley, and Lind (1987) found a negative correlation
between procedural justice and performance. Keller and Dansereau
(1995) found a moderately strong relationship between procedural
justice and performance as measured by performance appraisal
records. Other studies have linked distributive justice to perfor­
mance, consistent with equity theory’s predictions (e.g., Ball et al.,
1994; Griffeth, Vecchio, & Logan, 1989). It is difficult to apply the
logic of the agent-system and two-factor models to the prediction
of performance. On the one hand, performance supports, and is
often measured by, agents such as one’s supervisor. For this
reason, Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000) predicted, and found,
stronger interactional justice effects on performance, consistent
with the agent-system model. On the other hand, performance
reflects members’ contributions to organizational goals (Borman,
1991), giving it a system-referenced character and suggesting that
procedural justice should be its primary predictor.

Different Operationalizations of Procedural Justice

Of course, the relative strength of the correlations between the
justice dimensions and the outcomes just described may depend on
how procedural justice is operationalized. For example, some
studies assess only process control but label it procedural justice
(e.g., Joy & Witt, 1992). Others use a variable that is composed
only of Leventhal criteria, labeling that procedural justice (e.g.,
Konovsky & Folger, 1991). Still others measure informational or
interpersonal justice as the process variable (e.g., Greenberg,
1990a). Another common operationalization is measuring proce­
dural fairness perceptions (e.g., Bies, Martin, & Brockner, 1993;
Colquitt & Chertkoff, 1996; Gilliland, 1994), which is what Lind
and Tyler (1988) labeled a direct measure (i.e., a measure that
directly asks respondents “how fair” a procedure is). Finally,
another common operationalization is an indirect combination
measure (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Such a measure asks respondents
about some combination of process control, Leventhal criteria, and
interpersonal or informational justice, labeling that procedural
justice (e.g., Brockner et al., 1995, 1997; Folger & Konovsky,
1989; Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998;
Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). Presumably, those who use such
measures subscribe to the view that interpersonal and informa­
tional justice are facets of procedural justice, as with process
control or Leventhal criteria.

META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 431

Our examination of the relative strength of the correlations
between the different dimensions of justice and the outcomes
described uses the broadest conceptualization of procedural justice
by combining all of its potential operationalizations. However, our
review also presents analyses broken down by operationalization.
One strength of meta-analysis is that it allows a researcher to
examine the degree to which a relationship varies across a partic­
ular moderator variable. Our review examines operationalization
as a moderator variable, allowing us to see to what extent rela­
tionships vary by how procedural justice is conceptualized. For
instance, one might expect that procedural fairness perceptions
will yield the highest relationships with organizational outcomes,
in that fairness perceptions could be construed as the intervening
mechanism linking process control, Leventhal criteria, and so forth
to outcomes. Conversely, one might expect indirect combination
measures to yield the highest relationships, because they capture
more of the conceptual domain of organizational justice.

Method

We conducted a meta-analytic review of the literature on organizational
justice to examine the questions discussed earlier. In the following sec­
tions, we review the methods for this review.

Literature Search

We first performed a literature search of the PsycINFO database for the
years 1975 to 1999. Our starting date coincided with the year in which
Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the procedural justice construct.
Given that a major goal of this review was to examine the relative impact
of multiple dimensions of justice, articles published before 1975 would be
less useful. Such articles were typically proactive distributive justice arti­
cles that examined how individuals allocate rewards across different situ­
ations and did not normally assess any of the outcomes examined in this
review. Moreover, from a practical perspective, articles published before
1975 rarely reported the types of statistics included in a meta-analysis. We
conducted our literature search using the following keywords: procedural
fairness, procedural justice, distributive fairness, distributive justice, inter­
actional justice, interpersonal treatment, and equity. In addition to the
PsycINFO search, we conducted an author search of 10 of the most
published authors in the organizational justice literature. Any article that
included a relationship in the meta-analysis, whether it was a single
justice-outcome relationship or a relationship between two dimensions of
justice, was relevant.

The search yielded 300 articles. Of these 300 articles, 76 did not include
a relationship in the meta-analysis and were deemed not relevant. For
example, Tyler, Lind, Ohbuchi, Sugawara, and Huo (1998) examined the
effects of ‘‘relational judgments” (an aggregate measure of procedural,
interpersonal, and informational justice judgments) on the willingness of
disputants to accept a third-party solution in a conflict resolution setting.
Bazerman, Schroth, Shah, Diekmann, and Tenbrunsel (1994) examined the
effects of justice on job search decisions. Such studies did not focus on one
of the nine outcome variables included in our review or did not include a
relationship between different dimensions of justice.

Another 41 studies included a relationship in the meta-analysis but could
not be coded because of the way in which the results were presented.
Meta-analysis requires zero-order effect size information, whether in the
form of correlations, F statistics, t statistics, or even means and standard
deviations of a dependent variable across multiple experimental conditions.
Articles that present only partial or semipartial unique, independent effects
cannot be coded unless the independent variables are uncorrelated, as in
most experiments in which conditions are manipulated orthogonal to one
another. Several articles uncovered in our literature review could not be

coded because they presented only multiple regression or structural equa­
tion modeling results. Examples included several of Tyler’s investigations
of the “relational” or “group-value” model of justice (e.g., Tyler, 1989,
1994; Tyler, Degoey, & Smith, 1996). Other articles could not be coded
because they presented incomplete results (e.g., only labeling F statistics in
an analysis of variance as significant or not significant) or combined
procedural and distributive justice into a larger justice variable (e.g.,
Martocchio & Judge, 1995).

The final number of studies included in the review was 183. These
studies are marked with an asterisk in the References section. We should
note that this number included both laboratory studies and field studies; the
term organizational justice in no way implies that the research must occur
in a field setting but indicates only that the research must be relevant to the
role of fairness in the workplace (Greenberg, 1990b).

Meta-Analysis Strategy

Meta-analysis is a technique that allows individual study results to be
aggregated while correcting for various artifacts that can bias relationship
estimates. Our meta-analyses were conducted with Hunter and Schmidt’s
(1990) procedures. Inputs into the meta-analyses included zero-order effect
sizes in the form of correlations, along with sample sizes and reliability
information. In instances in which articles reported statistics other than
correlations (e.g., F statistics, t statistics, or means and standard devia­
tions), results were transformed into correlations through the formulas
provided by Hunter and Schmidt (1990). Because of the subjectivity and
judgment calls inherent in meta-analytic efforts, all coding of meta-analytic
data was performed by author dyads formed from the study’s five authors.

In cases in which a variable was assessed with multiple measures, we
acted in accordance with Hunter and Schmidt’s (1990) recommendations
for conceptual replication (see pp. 451-463). Specifically, when multiple
measures were highly correlated and seemed to each be construct valid, the
correlations were averaged together (see Hunter & Schmidt, 1990, p. 457).
Thus, one composite correlation was formed in lieu of multiple correla­
tions, preventing a study that involved multiple measures from being
“double counted.” This technique improves both reliability and construct
validity. We should also note that, when an article reported results from
multiple independent samples, each correlation was included in the meta­
analysis (see Hunter & Schmidt, 1990, pp. 463-466, for a discussion of
these issues).

Meta-analysis requires that each observed correlation from a given study
be weighted by that study’s sample size to provide a weighted mean
estimate of the population correlation. The standard deviation of this
estimate across the multiple studies is also computed. This variation is
composed of true variation in the population value as well as variation due
to artifacts such as sampling error and measurement error. To provide a
more accurate estimate of each population correlation and its variability,
our analyses corrected for both sampling error and unreliability. However,
because reliability information was not always available, the method of
artifact-distribution meta-analysis was used (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). For
those studies that did not report reliability information for any given
variable, that variable’s weighted mean reliability based on all other studies
was used to correct for measurement error.

Meta-analysis results typically include both uncorrected (r) and cor­
rected (rc) estimates of the population correlation. The latter are corrected
for unreliability in both variables. Also reported are the 95% confidence
intervals for each population correlation. Confidence intervals are gener­
ated with the standard error of the weighted mean correlation. They reflect
the “extent to which sampling error remains in the estimate of a mean
effect size” and are applied to estimates that have not been corrected for
artifacts (Whitener, 1990, p. 316). If a confidence interval does not include
the value of zero, that population correlation is judged to be “statistically
significant.”

Also presented is the standard deviation of the population correlation
(SDrc). This provides an index of the variation in the corrected population

432 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG

values across the studies in our sample. One indication that moderators
may be present in a given relationship is the case in which artifacts such as
unreliability fail to account for a substantial portion of the variance in
correlations. Thus, the percentage of variance explained by artifacts (VOT)
is also presented. Hunter and Schmidt (1990) have suggested that, if
artifacts fail to account for 75% of the variance in the correlations,
moderators probably exist. Mathieu and Zajac (1990) and Hom, Caranikas­
Walker, Prussia, and Griffeth (1992) amended the 75% criterion to 60% in
cases in which range restriction is not one of the artifacts that is corrected
for. We should note that the 60% rule only implies the existence of a
moderator; it does not indicate what variable is acting as the moderator.
Identifying the actual moderator variable requires coding potential mod­
erators and then performing subgroup analyses to determine whether the
population correlations vary across the subgroup boundaries. In our review,
this entailed determining whether the population correlations varied across
different operationalizations of procedural justice.

Meta-Analytic Regression Strategy

Many questions cannot be answered by a matrix of meta-analyzed
correlations. For example, do Leventhal’s (1980) justice criteria explain
variance in procedural fairness perceptions beyond the effects of Thibaut
and Walker’s (1975) process control construct? Do procedural, distribu­
tive, interpersonal, and informational justice each have independent effects
on organizational commitment? Such questions require multiple regression
to assess independent, semipartial effects. Fortunately, recent advances in
meta-analytic regression have allowed researchers to combine the benefits
of meta-analysis with the strengths of regression procedures (see Viswes­
varan & Ones, 1995). Meta-analytic regression has been used in contexts
such as training motivation (Colquitt, LePine, & Noe, 2000), leadership
(Podsakoff, MacKenzie, & Bommer, 1996), turnover (Hom et al., 1992),
and job performance (Schmidt, Hunter, & Outerbridge, 1986).

In practice, there are some decision points that researchers using meta-
analytic regression will encounter (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). First,
many researchers will conduct regressions on correlation matrices that vary
in sample size. This raises the question of what sample size to use when
computing the standard errors associated with the regression weights.
Potential solutions include using the smallest cell sample size or using the
mean sample size. We chose to use the harmonic mean of the correlation
matrix sample sizes, as opposed to the arithmetic mean (consistent with
Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). The formula for the harmonic mean is
[k/(lW1 + 1W2 + ■·· + 1/AJ], where к refers to the number of study
correlations and N refers to the sample sizes of the studies. An inspection
of the formula shows that the harmonic mean gives much less weight to
substantially large individual study sample sizes, so it is always more
conservative than the arithmetic mean (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). Note,
however, that the sample size issue is not very critical to the current
meta-analytic review. This is because the sample sizes for almost all of the
cells of the correlation matrices are in the thousands, making even small
effect sizes statistically significant, no matter what sample size option is
used.

Second, researchers must choose whether to use maximum-likelihood
estimation (the choice of Hom et al., 1992), ordinary least squares (the
choice of Colquitt et al., 2000, Podsakoff et al., 1996, and Schmidt et al.,
1986), or some other method. We elected to use ordinary least squares
estimation, consistent with Colquitt et al. (2000), Podsakoff et al. (1996),
and Schmidt et al. (1986). Ordinary least squares assumptions are less
restrictive than maximum likelihood, which assumes multivariate normal­
ity. Maximum-likelihood estimation is also less optimal in instances in
which the data are in the form of correlations rather than covariances
(Cudeck, 1989). Given that meta-analysis results in correlational data, we
believed that ordinary least squares would be more appropriate.

Results
Construct Discrimination Questions

Research Question I concerned the magnitude of the relation­
ships among the various organizational justice conceptualizations.
Table 1 shows the correlations among process control, Leventhal
criteria, interpersonal justice, informational justice, distributive
justice, and procedural fairness perceptions. Cohen and Cohen
(1983) classified correlations as high, moderate, or weak according
to uncorrected r values of .50, .30, and .10, respectively. Thus,
Table 1 shows high correlations among the justice conceptualiza­
tions, but not so high that they seem to be multiple indicators of
one underlying construct. Process control and Leventhal criteria,
the two original operationalizations of procedural justice, were
highly correlated with each other (r = .50, rc = .67) and with
procedural fairness perceptions (r = .41, rc = .51 and r = .53, rc =
.68, respectively). However, the relationship between Leventhal
criteria and procedural fairness perceptions was significantly
stronger than the relationship between process control and proce­
dural fairness perceptions, as evidenced by the fact that the two
correlations’ confidence intervals did not overlap.

Table 1 also shows the correlations among the interactional
justice dimensions. Interpersonal justice was highly related to
informational justice (r = .57, rc = .66), although again not so
highly that the two necessarily seem to be indicators of the same
underlying construct. Interpersonal justice and informational jus­
tice were also highly correlated with procedural fairness percep­
tions (r = .56, rc = .63 and r = .51, rc = .58, respectively). These
relationships were similar in magnitude to the process control and
Leventhal relationships, although the interpersonal justice­
procedural fairness relationship was significantly stronger than the
process control-procedural fairness relationship.

Interestingly, the relationships between interpersonal and infor­
mational justice and procedural fairness perceptions were not
significantly stronger than the relationship between distributive
justice and procedural fairness perceptions (r = .48, rc = .57).
This suggests that interpersonal and informational justice should
be considered to be distinct from procedural justice, just as the case
with distributive justice. The uncorrected correlation of .48 for
distributive justice and procedural fairness perceptions may seem
surprisingly low, given the many empirical articles that have
reported uncorrected relationships in the .60s and .70s. To take a
closer look at this relationship. Table 2 shows relationships be­
tween distributive justice and every possible conceptualization of
procedural justice, including interpersonal and informational jus­
tice, to explore whether operationalization moderates the proce­
dural justice-distributive justice relationship. The top row of Ta­
ble 2, labeled “Broadly defined procedural justice,” shows the
procedural justice-distributive relationship in which all potential
operationalizations, shown in the remaining rows, are considered
together. The final row contains the “indirect combination mea­
sure” operationalization discussed previously. Recall that indirect
combination measures are those that assess some combination of
process control, interpersonal or informational justice, and Lev­
enthal variables. They are labeled indirect because they do not
directly ask “how fair” something is (see Lind & Tyler, 1988).

Combining all operationalizations, broadly defined procedural
justice was strongly related to distributive justice (r = .56, rc =
.67). The high standard deviation of the population correlation

META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 433
Ta

bl
e

1
Re

la
tio

ns
hi

ps
A

m
on

g
D

iff
er

en
t C

on
ce

pt
ua

liz
at

io
ns

o
f O

rg
an

iz
at

io
na

l J
us

tic
e

ä

«ï
c
о

ω Q G 05

W 73
C
сЗ

«’S
G
ОC

О
О

о
t
с

«л
d
О

-2

ë
О

оз
3
а
о

&
о
ä

э
а
«3

с
о

я

— 73
Я 5
S* о _
2″ л

(SDrc = .23) and the low variance explained from artifacts
(Vatt = 2.74%) show that moderators exist in the broadly defined
procedural justice-distributive justice relationship. Operational­
ization explains some of that variation, in that the highest relation­
ship was evident when an indirect combination measure was used
0 = .63, r. = .77). Moreover, because their confidence intervals
do not overlap, Table 2 shows that the indirect combination mea­
sure relationship is significantly stronger than the more unidimen­
sional justice measures: procedural fairness perceptions (r = .48,
rc = .57), process control (r = .27, rc = .34), Leventhal criteria
(r = .46, r, = .55), interpersonal justice (r = .38, rc = .42), and
informational justice (r = .39, rc = .46).

Proactive Research Questions

Research Question 2 explored whether the additional conceptu­
alizations of justice over the course of its history have contributed
incremental variance explained in regard to promoting procedural
fairness perceptions. This was tested by regressing procedural
fairness perceptions onto the various justice operationalizations
using hierarchical regression. Order of entry was based on histor­
ical introduction, so the following steps were used: (a) Thibaut and
Walker’s (1975) process control operationalization, (b) Lev­
enthal’s (1980) justice criteria operationalization, and (c) Bies and
Moag’s (1986) interactional justice construct, broken down into
interpersonal and informational justice (as in Greenberg, 1993b).

The meta-analytic regression results are shown in Table 3. The
original operationalization of procedural justice, process control,
explained 26% of the variance in procedural fairness perceptions,
with higher levels of process control producing more favorable
fairness perceptions (ß = .51). Leventhal criteria explained an
additional 21% of the variance, with higher levels of his criteria
also producing more favorable perceptions (ß = .61). Interper­
sonal and informational justice explained an additional 6% of the
variance in fairness perceptions. That effect was due primarily to
interpersonal justice (ß = .29), although informational justice also
had a significant effect (ß = .11).

Research Question 3 asked how much variance the justice
operationalizations explained in procedural fairness perceptions
beyond the effects of distributive justice. This was tested by
regressing procedural fairness perceptions onto the various justice
operationalizations after controlling for distributive justice. These
regression results are shown in Table 4. Distributive justice ex­
plained 33% of the variance in procedural fairness perceptions,
with higher levels of distributive justice being associated with
more favorable fairness perceptions (ß = .57). The remaining
justice operationalizations explained an incremental 24% of the
variance. However, only two operationalizations had strong unique
effects: Leventhal criteria (ß = .30) and interpersonal justice (ß =
.26). Although statistically significant, the unique effects of pro­
cess control (ß = .03) and informational justice (ß = .07) were not
practically significant.

Reactive Research Questions

Research Questions 4-6 explored the relationships between the
organizational justice dimensions and several key outcome vari­
ables. Table 5 presents the results for these research questions.
Research Question 4 explored the relationships between distribu-

434 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG

Table 2
Relationship Between Distributive and Procedural Justice Conceptualizations
(Including Interpersonal and Informational Justice)

Conceptualization

Distributive justice

r (95% CI) N (SDrO k (N) Vm (%)

Broadly defined procedural justice .56 (.52, .60) .67 (.23) 92 (42,576) 2.74
Procedural fairness perceptions .48 (.41, .55) .57 (.28) 45(13,418) 3.41
Process control .27 (.22, .33) .34 (.13) 23(5,137) 24.15
Leventhal criteria .46 (.38, .54) .55 (.17) 15 (4,743) 8.27
Interpersonal justice .38 (.26, .50) .42 (.20) 9 (3,496) 5.62
informational justice .39 (.32, .47) .46 (.15) 14 (3,807) 13.19
Indirect combination measure .63 (.59, .66) .77 (.14) 54 (51,446) 2.32

Note, r = uncorrected population correlation; Cl = confidence interval around uncorrected population
correlation; rc = corrected population correlation; SDrc = standard deviation of corrected population correlation;
к = number of studies; Vart = percentage of variance in rc explained by study artifacts.

tive justice and the outcomes. Distributive justice had high corre­
lations with outcome satisfaction (r = .52, rc — .61), job satisfac­
tion (r = .46, rc = .56), organizational commitment (r = .42, r,. =
.51), trust (r = .48, rc = .57), agent-referenced evaluation of
authority (r = .53, = .59), and withdrawal (r = —.41, rc =
—.50). Distributive justice had moderate correlations with system-
referenced evaluation of authority (r = .30, rc = .37), OCBOs (r =
.20, rc = .25), and negative reactions (r = —.26, ra = —.30) and
was weakly related to OCBIs (r = .13, rc = .15) and performance
(r = .13, rc = .15).

Research Question 5 explored relationships between procedural
justice and the same set of outcomes. Combining all conceivable
procedural justice conceptualizations, again labeled broadly de­
fined procedural justice, we see that procedural justice had high
correlations with outcome satisfaction (r = .40, rc = .48), job
satisfaction (r = .51, rc = .62), organizational commitment (r =
.48, rc = .57), trust (r = .52, rc = .61), and agent-referenced
evaluation of authority (r = .56, rc = .64). Procedural justice had
moderate correlations with system-referenced evaluation of au­
thority (r = .35, rc = .42), OCBOs (r = .23, rc = .27), withdrawal
(r = —.36, re = —.46), negative reactions (r = —.Ti, rc = —.31),
and performance (r = .30, = .36). Finally, procedural justice
had weak correlations with OCBIs (r = .19, rc = .22). Table 5 also
shows that moderators were present in the procedural justice­
outcome relationships, with the exception of the relationships with
the OCB variables. Operationalization could be one potential mod­
erator for several of the outcome variables, because the indirect

Table 3
Incremental Effects of Justice Operationalizations on
Procedural Fairness Perceptions

Regression step Total R2 Δ/?2 В
1. Process control .26* .26* .51*
2. Leventhal criteria .47* .21* .61*
3. Interpersonal justice .53* .06* .28*

Informational justice .11*

Note. N – 4,165 (based on the harmonic mean sample size of the Table 1
cells used in this regression).
*p< .05. combination measure was significantly more related to job satis­ faction, organizational commitment, and withdrawal than was any other operationalization. Research Question 6 explored relationships between interper­ sonal and informational justice and the outcome variables. Inter­ personal justice was strongly related to agent-referenced evalua­ tion of authority (r = .57, rc = .62) and moderately related to job satisfaction (r = .31, rc = .35), system-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .20, rc = .23), OCBIs (r = .23, rc = .29), and negative reactions (r = —.30, rc = —.35). It was weakly related to outcome satisfaction (r = .19), organizational commitment (r = . 16, rc = .19), withdrawal (r = —.02, rc = —.02), and performance (r = .03, r^ = .03). Informational justice was strongly related to trust (r = .43, rc = .51), agent-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .58, rc = .65), and system-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .42, r = .47) and was moderately related to outcome satisfaction (r = .27, rc = .30), job satisfaction (r = .38, rc = .43), organizational commitment (r = .26, r^ = .29), OCBIs (r = .21, rc = .26), withdrawal (r = —.21, re = —.24), and negative reactions (r = — .29, rc = — .33). It was weakly related to OCBOs (r = .18) and performance (r = .11, rc = .13). Research Question 7 examined the unique effects of the justice dimensions on the outcome variables considered simultaneously. Table 6 presents beta weights derived from regressing each of the outcomes on all four justice dimensions simultaneously. The table also presents unique R2 values derived from a “usefulness analy­ sis” in which the effects of one justice dimension were examined after controlling for the others. The table also presents the total variance explained in each outcome by the justice dimensions. Note that, in some cases, the interpersonal justice beta weights were reversed in sign from that dimension’s correlations. This was due to the multicollinearity in some of the regressions. If hierar­ chical regression had been used and interpersonal justice had been entered in a step before procedural and informational justice were entered, the signs of the beta weights would not have been reversed. The regression results in Table 6 allowed us to examine the adequacy of the distributive dominance, two-factor, and agent­ system reactive models. Leventhal’s (1980) distributive domi­ nance model would predict higher unique effects for distribu- META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 435 Table 4 Incremental Effects of Justice Operationalizations on Procedural Fairness Perceptions Beyond Distributive Justice Effects Regression step Total R2 Δ«2 В 1. Distributive justice .33* .33* .57* 2. Process control .57* .24* .03* Leventhal criteria .30* Interpersonal justice .26* Informational justice .07* Note. N = 4,165 (based on the harmonic mean sample size of the Table 1 cells used in this regression). * p < .05. tive justice than for the other three justice forms. This prediction was supported for two outcomes (outcome satisfac­ tion and withdrawal) and was refuted for the remaining nine outcomes. Sweeney and McFarlin’s (1993) two-factor model would predict higher unique effects of distributive justice for person-referenced outcomes and higher unique effects of procedural justice for system-referenced outcomes. This prediction was supported for five outcomes (outcome satisfaction, job satisfaction, organiza­ tional commitment, system-referenced evaluation of authority, and performance) and was refuted for three outcomes (OCBOs, with­ drawal, and negative reactions). The two-factor model was not relevant to the three agent-referenced outcomes (agent-referenced evaluation of authority, trust, and OCBIs). Bies and Moag’s (1986) agent-system model would predict higher unique effects of interpersonal or informational justice for agent-referenced outcomes and higher unique effects of procedural justice for system-referenced outcomes. This prediction was sup­ ported for five outcomes (job satisfaction, organizational commit­ ment, agent-referenced evaluation of authority, OCBIs, and per­ formance) and refuted for five outcomes (system-referenced evaluation of authority, trust, OCBOs, withdrawal, and negative reactions). The agent-system model was not relevant to person- referenced outcomes such as outcome satisfaction. Discussion This meta-analytic review, consisting of 120 separate meta­ analyses of 183 empirical studies, explored three types of research questions to aid further theory development in the organizational justice literature. The first type of question was related to construct discrimination (i.e., how highly related are the various facets of organizational justice?). Process control and Leventhal criteria, the two original procedural justice operationalizations, were highly correlated, although perhaps not as highly as one would think given that they are used interchangeably to express the same construct. Similarly, interpersonal and informational justice were highly correlated, but again not so highly that it seems prudent to lump them together under the “interactional justice” label. Indeed, their correlation was not significantly higher than the correlation between procedural justice and distributive justice, two constructs whose separation has become canon. Further analyses showed that the procedural justice-distributive justice relationship varies to some degree by how the researcher operationalizes the former. The relationship is strongest when an indirect combination measure is used and weaker when procedural fairness perceptions, Leventhal criteria, and especially process control are used. The second type of research question dealt with proactive re­ search, which is concerned with creating procedural fairness per­ ceptions by adhering to certain rules, such as providing process control, being consistent, treating people with sensitivity, or ex­ plaining things adequately. Our results showed that the historical progression of proactive research on procedural justice has in fact contributed to our ability to promote procedural fairness percep­ tions. The original conceptualization, Thibaut and Walker’s (1975) process control, predicted procedural fairness perceptions, but Leventhal’s (1980) criteria contributed significant incremental variance. Indeed, it is worth noting that the Leventhal criteria had a significantly stronger relationship to procedural fairness percep­ tions than did process control. In fact, the Leventhal criteria are even more impressive when we consider that they predicted almost as much variance in procedural fairness perceptions as process control, even when entered in a later step in the regression analysis. The concepts of interpersonal and informational justice, drawn from the field’s recent interest in interactional justice, show a more mixed pattern of results. Their correlations with procedural fair­ ness perceptions were just as strong as the correlations for Lev­ enthal criteria. However, although they explained significant in­ cremental variance in fairness perceptions, their contribution was small in comparison with the contributions of the other justice facets. Thus, interpersonal and informational justice, when con­ sidered alone, were powerful predictors of procedural fairness perceptions. When considered in conjunction with structural facets of procedural justice, their contribution was less important, partic­ ularly in the case of informational justice. Perhaps this is not surprising considering that informational justice often provides the opportunity to judge structural qualities of procedural justice (Greenberg, 1993b). Because it is well known that the fairness of decision-making procedures is often judged according to the kinds of outcomes one receives (Lind & Tyler, 1988), it is also important to show that the various conceptualizations of procedural justice are important even after control for distributive justice. If this had not been the case, then the procedural justice literature would have contributed little over earlier work by Homans (1961), Adams (1965), Leventhal (1976), and Deutsch (1975). Our results showed that, when dis­ tributive justice was controlled, only Leventhal criteria and inter­ personal justice retained their explanatory power. Because process control added little, it is tempting to conclude that measuring it is unnecessary if distributive justice and Leventhal criteria are also being considered. Perhaps this is not surprising given Lind and Tyler’s (1988) assertion that Leventhal’s “representativeness” cri­ terion includes the process control concept. Turning to our third type of research question, reactive research, we tested three separate reactive models: Leventhal’s (1980) dis­ tributive dominance model, Sweeney and McFarlin’s (1993) two- factor model, and Bies and Moag’s (1986) agent-system model. Support for these models can be evaluated by examining the relative effects of distributive, procedural, interpersonal, and in­ formational justice on the basis of the size of their meta-analytic correlations, as well as their unique effects in the meta-analytic regressions. We find little support for the distributive dominance 436 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG Table 5 Relationships Between Organizational Justice Dimensions and Outcome Variables Dimension r (95% CI) % «DrJ k (N) van(%) r (95% CI) % W к IN) vart (%) Outcome satisfaction Job satisfaction Broadly defined procedural justice .40 (.35, .46) .48 (.18) 30 (8,073) 10.02 .51 (.46, .56) .62 (.18) 40 (31,774) 2.88 Procedural fairness perceptions .45 (.36, .54) .53 (.17) 11 (4,420) 6.59 .33 (.28, .38) .40 (.09) 11 (4,958) 24.00 Process control .38 (.27, .48) .45 (.16) 8(1,774) 14.74 .30 (.25, .35) .37 (.05) 5 (2,577) 50.39 Leventhal criteria .25 (.13, .37) .32 (.17) 6(1,796) 13.03 .36 (.33, .40) .42 (.00) 4(2,315) 100.00 Interpersonal justice .19 1 (301) .31 (.26,.36) .35 (.02) 2(1,795) 65.96 Informational justice .27 (.21, .33) .30 (.04) 4(1,404) 65.02 .38 (.34, .42) .43 (.00) 2(1,872) 100.00 Indirect combination measure .47 (.39, .54) .54 (.15) 14 (2,242) 18.87 .55 (.49, .61) .68 (.17) 22(25,221) 2.07 Distributive justice .52 (.46, .59) .61 (.20) 28 (9,321) 5.07 .46 (.42, .49) .56 (.09) 24 (57,515) 8.31 Organizational commitment Trust Broadly defined procedural justice .48 (.44, .52) .57 (.18) 53 (33,455) 3.84 .52 (.44, .59) .61 (.20) 24 (4,522) 8.42 Procedural fairness perceptions .32 (.25, .39) .37 (.16) 18 (6,767) 9.58 .56 (.49, .64) .62 (.08) 7 (802) 39.78 Process control .22 (.18, .27) .27 (.05) 7 (2,898) 53.50 .40 (.30, .50) .47 (.13) 7(1,031) 26.54 Leventhal criteria .26 (.22, .29) .30 (.00) 5 (3,162) 100.00 .58 (.09, .99) .65 (.38) 2 (628) 1.23 Interpersonal justice .16 (.11, .20) .19 (.00) 2(1,824) 100.00 Informational justice .26 (.18, .34) .29 (.14) 10 (3,968) 12.01 .43 (.30, .57) .51 (.11) 3 (487) 30.21 Indirect combination measure .55 (.51, .58) .65 (.10) 26 (24,606) 6.10 .55 (.45, .66) .64 (.18) 10(2,169) 8.02 Distributive justice .42 (.38, .47) .51 (.13) 24 (27,805) 4.11 .48 (.40, .57) .57 (.13) 8 (1,735) 17.24 Evaluation of authority: Agent referenced Evaluation of authority: System referenced Broadly defined procedural justice .56 (.52, .59) .64 (.11) 33 (20,034) 7.17 .35 (-31,-40) .42 (.13) 25 (9,708) 13.09 Procedural fairness perceptions .53 (.46, .60) .60 (.13) 13 (4,753) 8.76 .45 (.42, .48) .51 (.12) 14 (5,637) 11.63 Process control .42 (.35, .50) .50 (.14) 10 (3,436) 12.56 .12 (.05, .20) .16 (.00) 3(663) 100.00 Leventhal criteria .53 (.41, .64) .63 (.17) 7 (3,104) 4.86 .19 (.02, .36) .22 (.15) 3 (427) 29.27 Interpersonal justice .57 (.43, .71) .62 (.17) 5 (2,534) 3.44 .20 (.01, .38) .23 (.16) 3 (469) 22.95 Informational justice .58 (.46, .71) .65 (.21) 9 (3,210) 3.21 .42 (.29, .55) .47 (.15) 5(1,035) 14.94 Indirect combination measure .58 (.54, .62) .67 (.05) 9 (13,850) 6.65 .31 (.24, .38) .37 (.12) 11 (3,790) 17.96 Distributive justice .53 (.46, .61) .59 (.15) 13 (16,963) 1.96 .30 (.24, .36) .37 (.12) 13 (4,103) 20.64 OCBs: Individual referenced OCBs: Organization referenced Broadly defined procedural justice .19 (.16, .22) .22 (.00) 15 (4,414) 100.00 .23 (.19, .26) .27 (.04) 15(3,176) 78.62 Procedural fairness perceptions .21 (.17, .25) .25 (.00) 2(1,872) 100.00 .21 1 (140) Process control .16 (.12, .20) .21 (.00) 3 (2,000) 100.00 .14 1 (206) Leventhal criteria .18 (.14, .23) .22 (.01) 3 (2,108) 85.39 .15 (.06, .24) .18 (.00) 2(431) 100.00 Interpersonal justice .23 (.18, .27) .29 (.00) 2(1,794) 100.00 Informational justice .21 (.17, .26) .26 (.00) 2(1,883) 100.00 .18 1 (206) Indirect combination measure .19 (.14, .23) .22 (.02) 10(1,994) 90.18 .25 (.20, .30) .30 (.06) 9(1,961) 63.06 Distributive justice .13 (.09, .17) .15 (.02) 6 (2,633) 86.57 .20 (.14, .26) .25 (.00) 5 (903) 100.00 Withdrawal Negative reactions Broadly defined procedural justice -.36 (-.42, -.31) -.46 (.20) 39 (24,273) 4.12 -.27 (-.33, -.21) -.31 (.18) 27 (6,275) 13.37 Procedural fairness perceptions -.27 (-.32, -.21) -.34 (.14) 21 (7,344) 14.46 -.33 (-.39, -.26) -.38 (.12) 13 (3,563) 22.15 Process control -.19 (-.27, -.10) -.24 (.14) 10(1,190) 39.94 -.23 (-.27, -.19) -.30 (.00) 3 (2,094) 100.00 Leventhal criteria -.23 (-.30, -.16) -.29 (.07) 6(1,431) 50.87 -.28 (-.35, -.22) -.35 (.06) 5 (2,308) 34.76 Interpersonal justice -.02 (-.13, .09) -.02 (.00) 2(316) 100.00 -.30 (-.35, -.26) -.35 (.04) 7 (2,707) 58.78 Informational justice -.21 (-.34, -.07) -.24 (.21) 8(1,692) 11.42 -.29 (-.34, -.24) -.33 (.06) 8(2,731) 45.25 Indirect combination measure -.44 (-.56, -.32) -.55 (.19) 6 (14,392) 1.11 -.20 (-.35, -.05) -.22 (.21) 7(1,807) 9.28 Distributive justice -.41 (-.46, -.37) -.50 (.12) 18(15,888) 7.45 -.26 (-.35, -.17) -.30 (.17) 13 (3,782) 12.27 Performance Broadly defined procedural justice .30 (.21,-39) .36 (.29) 30 (8,317) 4.83 Procedural fairness perceptions .30 (.17, .43) .36 (.33) 18 (6,925) 2.71 Process control .14 (-.01,-29) .17 (.25) 8(1,002) 16.17 Leventhal criteria .08 (-.05, .21) ■ 10(.ll) 3 (501) 44.34 Interpersonal justice .03 (-.14, .20) .03 (.11) 2 (389) 34.78 Informational justice .11 (.00, .22) .13 (.11) 4(1,036) 29.79 Indirect combination measure .23 (.12, .35) .27 (.00) 7 (1,084) 23.83 Distributive justice .13 (.03, .22) .15 (.18) 13(2,294) . 18.43 Note. r = uncorrected population correlation; CI = confidence interval around uncorrected population correlation; r. = corrected population correlation; SDr = standard deviation of corrected population correlation; к = number of studies; Vart = percentage of variance in rc explained by study artifacts; OCBs = organizational citizenship behaviors. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 437 Table 6 Unique Effects of Procedural, Interpersonal, Informational, and Distributive Justice on Outcome Variables Outcome variable Justice dimension Total R2Procedural Interpersonal Informational Distributive Outcome satisfaction (N = 1,792) ß .17* -.08* .02 .54* Unique R2 .02* .00* .00 .18* .39* Job satisfaction (N = 4,039) ß .48* -.09* .13* .26* Unique R2 .11* .01* .01* .04* .45* Organizational commitment (N = 4,582) ß .42* -.18* .07* .31* Unique R2 .09* .02* .00* .06* .35* Evaluation of authority: Agent-referenced (N = 4,517) ß . .01 .27* .32* .32* Unique R2 .00 .04* .05* .06* .57* Evaluation of authority: System-referenced (N = 2,114) ß .30* -.19* .44* .11* Unique R2 .04* .02* .10* .01* .31* Trust (N = 1,711) ß Unique R2 .31* .05* .21* .03* .30* .05* .45* OCBs: Individual referenced (N = 3,192) ß .06* .19* .11* -.01* Unique R2 .00* .02* .01* .00 .09* OCBs: Organization referenced (N = 782) ß Unique R2 .12* .01* .11* .01* .12* .01* .08* Performance (N = 1,855) ß .56* -.20* .07* -.07 Unique R2 .15* .02* .00* .00* .19* Withdrawal (N = 1,919) ß -.10* .23* -.21* -.51* Unique R2 .01* .02* .02* .16* .33* Negative reactions (N = 4,039) ß -.06* -.18* -.12* -.14* Unique R2 .00* .02* .01* .01* .16* Note. Sample sizes are based on the harmonic mean of the sample sizes for the correlations among the justice and outcome variables. The procedural justice variable includes process control, Leventhal criteria, and procedural fairness perceptions. OCBs = organizational citizenship behaviors. * p < .05. model, which predicts that distributive justice will have stronger effects than the other justice dimensions. This model was sup­ ported for outcome satisfaction and withdrawal but not for any of the other nine outcomes. The two-factor model predicts that procedural justice will have stronger effects than distributive justice on system-referenced vari­ ables but weaker effects than distributive justice on person- referenced variables. This model seemed to receive support only for person-referenced and system-referenced attitudes such as out­ come satisfaction, job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and system-referenced evaluation of authority. The two-factor model’s predictions were not supported for more behavioral vari­ ables such as OCBs, withdrawal, and negative reactions. The only exception to this observation involved performance. Procedural justice was more capable of predicting performance than distrib­ utive justice, which supports the two-factor model if performance is assumed to be a system-referenced outcome. The agent-system model predicts that interpersonal or informa­ tional justice will have stronger effects than procedural justice on agent-referenced variables but weaker effects than procedural jus­ tice on system-referenced variables. This model was supported for agent-referenced outcomes, including agent-referenced evaluation of authority and OCBIs, but not for trust, which was more related to procedural and distributive justice. The agent-system model was also supported for job satisfaction, organizational commit­ ment, and performance. The model actually seems to underesti­ mate the importance of interpersonal or informational justice for behavioral variables. Interpersonal or informational justice was a strong predictor of OCBOs, withdrawal, and negative reactions, which would not have been predicted on the basis of the agent­ system model. Implications for the Organizational Justice Literature The conceptualization, measurement, and analysis of organiza­ tional justice depend in large part on a given study’s research question, as well as the sample or setting used to examine it. Nonetheless, the results of our review have some broad, general implications for the justice literature as a whole. Distinctiveness of justice dimensions. The construct discrimi­ nation results suggest that procedural, interpersonal, and informa­ tional justice are distinct constructs that can be empirically distin- 438 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG guished from one another. We would therefore call for a moratorium on indirect combination measures that combine the three justice dimensions into a single variable. Thirty-five of the studies included in our review used such measures (e.g., Brockner et al., 1995, 1997; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). Our review showed that procedural, interpersonal, and informa­ tional justice have different correlates, and measuring the three separately allows for further differences among the dimensions to be examined. Measurement of justice dimensions. It is also critical that researchers devote more care and effort to the measurement of justice dimensions. Some of the articles we reviewed used mea­ sures that assessed, among supervisors, granting subordinates voice, treating subordinates consistently, suppressing biases, being respectful, and providing explanations. Such measures were la­ beled “interactional justice” (e.g., Moorman, 1991; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997), even though only the latter two are included in Bies and Moag’s (1986) construct. Authors may have included such items on the basis of Folger and Bies’s (1989) article on “managerial responsibilities” for procedural justice, which listed seven principles (process control, bias suppression, consistency, feedback, justification, truthfulness, and courtesy) managers should use to promote procedural justice. Not surprisingly, re­ searchers using such measures have been forced to collapse the interactional justice measure with their procedural justice measure as a result of high intercorrelations (Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). Thus, more attention should be paid to content validity, as in a recent study by Colquitt (2001), who developed scales for the justice dimensions based on the seminal works introducing them and validated the scales in two indepen­ dent studies. ■ Distinguishing justice content from justice source. Relatedly, future research should seek to separate the effects of justice con­ tent from the effects of justice source. Procedural justice can be a function of an organization, as when a formalized decision-making system provides process control or consistency as a result of the way in which it is structured (e.g., Adams-Roy & Barling, 1998; Ehlen, Magner, & Welker, 1999; Skarlicki et al., 1999). Procedural justice can also be a function of a decision-making agent, as when a manager takes steps to involve subordinates in decisions or treat subordinates consistently (e.g., Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky, Folger, & Cropanzano, 1987; Korsgaard, Schweiger, & Sapienza, 1995). Likewise, informational and interpersonal justice can be fostered through organizational policies and initiatives (e.g., Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Tansky, 1993) or through the actions of a decision-making agent (e.g., Bies, Shapiro, & Cummings, 1988; Greenberg, 1994; Moorman, 1991). It is difficult to draw conclusions from studies comparing the effects of organization-originating procedural justice with agent-originating interpersonal or informational justice because source and content are confounded. Fortunately, recent research is beginning to examine this important issue by crossing justice source with justice content to assess their joint effects (Blader & Tyler, 2000; Byrne & Cropanzano, 2000; Masterson, Bartol, & Moye, 2000). Including multiple justice dimensions in single studies. Our results also clearly show that researchers can explain more out­ come variance by including multiple justice dimensions within single studies, although there are some diminishing returns asso­ ciated with this strategy. Our proactive research results showed that distributive justice, procedural justice (in terms of Leventhal criteria), interpersonal justice, and, to a lesser extent, informational justice each contribute uniquely to the creation of fairness percep­ tions. Our reactive research results showed that procedural and distributive justice are sufficient to adequately predict several outcomes, and procedural and distributive justice were either the strongest or second strongest predictors of 15 outcomes. However, interpersonal and informational justice clearly contributed to the prediction of other outcomes. For example, one or both of these variables were strong independent predictors of behavioral out­ comes such as OCBIs, OCBOs, withdrawal, and negative reac­ tions. Informational justice was the strongest predictor of both agent-referenced and system-referenced evaluation of authority. These results suggest that researchers interested in most evaluation or behavioral outcomes should assess both structural and interac­ tional facets of justice. Examining interactions among justice dimensions. In addition to explaining more outcome variance, including multiple dimen­ sions will also allow for the testing of interaction effects. The interaction between procedural and distributive justice has been perhaps the most robust finding in the justice literature (Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996). Skarlicki and Folger (1997) further showed that procedural and interactional justice interact in predicting ORBs. These authors also found support for three-way interactions among procedural, interactional, and distributive justice. Such complex relationships do not map neatly onto reactive models such as the two-factor or agent-system model. However, the fact that moderators were present in almost all of the justice-outcome relationships suggests that more complex relationships may be the key to improving outcome prediction. Gaps in the existing literature. A final implication of our results resides in the gaps revealed by this review. A scan of Table 5 highlights areas where more research ought to be done. In terms of sheer number of studies, we note that procedural justice is much better represented in studies of satisfaction, commitment, evaluation of authorities, withdrawal, and negative reactions and relatively underrepresented in studies of performance, OCBs, and trust. We also note that interpersonal and informational justice have received less attention than distributive and procedural jus­ tice, probably as a result of their more recent appearance in the justice literature. This is particularly evident for interpersonal justice, which has been assessed in 16 studies, as opposed to 31 studies for informational justice. As more researchers consider multiple justice dimensions in their work, these gaps should begin to be filled. Limitations This review has some limitations that should be noted. First, as in the primary studies on which our review is based, many of the variables were assessed with self-report measures. Thus, many of the relationships may be inflated because of same source bias. Second, any meta-analysis is subject to a variety of judgment calls META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 439 (Wanous, Sullivan, & Malinak, 1989). Although we performed all coding in author dyads, it is possible that some of these judgment calls affected our results. Finally, the fact that meta-analysis re­ quires the reporting of zero-order results meant that several im­ portant justice articles could not be included in our review. In particular, many of Tyler’s examinations of the relational model of justice had to be omitted (e.g., Tyler, 1989, 1994; Tyler et al., 1996). Conclusion In a review of the organizational justice literature, Greenberg (1993a) suggested that the field was in a state of “intellectual adolescence.” This adolescence, though marked by many advance­ ments, is also characterized by “stumbling awkwardness” due to underdeveloped research agendas and the absence of underlying theory (Greenberg, 1993a, p. 139). In an earlier review, Greenberg (1990b) echoed these sentiments, noting that the field was years away from the final stage of Reichers and Schneider’s (1990) construct life cycle. That final stage, termed “consolidation and accommodation,” is characterized by a reduction in controversies and an increase in agreement about definitions, antecedents, and consequences. Meta-analytic reviews, according to Greenberg (1990b), can help create such consolidation. Thus, we hope that the review presented in this article can help the field enter a more mature stage, as research on justice enters the new millennium. , References References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis. Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267-299). New York: Academic Press. *Adams-Roy, J., & Barling, J. (1998). Predicting the decision to confront or report sexual harassment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 329-336. Alexander, S., & Ruderman, Μ. (1987). The role of procedural and distributive justice in organizational behavior. Social Justice Re­ search, 1, 177-198. Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 1-18. *Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. (1989). The influence of social compar­ isons on perceptions of procedural fairness. Journal of Business and Psychology, 4, 129-138. *Aquino, K. (1995). Relationships among pay inequity, perceptions of procedural justice, and organizational citizenship. Employee Responsi­ bilities and Rights Journal, 8, 21-33. »Aquino, K., Griffeth, R. W., Allen, D. G., & Hom, P. W. (1997). Integrating justice constructs into the turnover process: A test of a referent cognitions model. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 1208­ 1227. »Armstrong-Stassen, Μ. (1998). The effect of gender and organizational level on how survivors appraise and cope with organizational downsiz­ ing. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 34, 125-142. »Austin, W., McGinn, N. C., & Susmilch, C. (1980). Internal standards revisited: Effects of social comparisons and expectancies on judgments of fairness and satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol­ ogy, 16, 426-441. »Ball, G. A., Trevino, L. K. & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1993). Justice and organizational punishment: Attitudinal outcomes of disciplinary events. Social Justice Research, 6, 39-67. »Ball, G. A., Trevino, L. K., & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1994). Just and unjust punishment: Influences on subordinate performance and citizenship. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 299-322. »Barclay, J. H., & Harland, L. K. (1995). Peer performance appraisals: The impact of rater competence, rater location, and rating correctability on fairness perceptions. Group and Organization Management, 20, 39-60. »Barling, J., & Phillips, Μ. (1993). Interactional, formal and distributive justice in the workplace: An exploratory study. Journal of Psychology, 127, 649-656. Bazerman, Μ. H., Schroth, H„ Shah, P., Diekmann, К., & Tenbrunsel, A. (1994). The inconsistent role of comparison others and procedural jus­ tice in reactions to hypothetical job descriptions; Implications for job acceptance decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 326-352. »Bennett, N., Martin, C. L., Bies, R. J., & Brockner, J. (1995). Coping with a layoff: A longitudinal study of victims. Journal of Management, 21, 1025-1040. »Bennett, R. J. (1998). Taking the sting out of the whip: Reactions to consistent punishment for unethical behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 248-262. »Bies, R. J. (1987). Beyond “voice”: The influence of decision-maker justification and sincerity on procedural fairness judgments. Represen­ tative Research in Social Psychology, 17, 3-14. »Bies, R. J., Martin, C. L., & Brockner, J. (1993). Just laid off, but still a “good citizen?” Only if the process is fair. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 6, 227-238. Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. F. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness. In R. J. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiations in organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 43-55). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. »Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (1987). Interactional fairness judgments: The influence of causal accounts. Social Justice Research, 1, 199-218. »Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (1988). Voice and justification: Their influence on procedural fairness judgments. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 676-685. »Bies, R. J., Shapiro, D. L., & Cummings, L. L. (1988). Causal accounts and managing organizational conflict: Is it enough to say it’s not my fault? Communication Research, 15, 381-399. Blader, S. L., & Tyler, T. R. (2000, August). A four-component model of procedural justice: What makes a process fair in work settings? Paper presented at the 59th Annual Meeting of the National Academy of Management, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley. »Bobocel, D. R., Agar, S. E., Meyer, J. P., & Irving, P. G. (1998). Managerial accounts and fairness perceptions in conflict resolution: Differentiating the effects of minimizing responsibility and providing justification. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 20, 133-143. »Bobocel, D. R., & Farrell, A. C. (1996). Sex-based promotion decisions and interactional fairness: Investigating the influence of managerial accounts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 22-35. Borman, W. C. (1991). Job behavior, performance, and effectiveness. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 271-326). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. »Brockner, J., DeWitt, R. L., Grover, S., & Reed, T. (1990). When it is especially important to explain why: Factors affecting the relationship between managers’ explanations of a layoff and survivors’ reactions to the layoff. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 389-407. »Brockner, J., Konovsky, Μ., Cooper-Schneider, R., Folger, R., Martin, C., & Bies, R. J. (1994). Interactive effects of procedural justice and 440 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG outcome negativity on victims and survivors of job loss. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 397-409. *Brockner, L, Siegel, P. A., Daly, J. P., & Martin, C. (1997). When trust matters: The moderating effect of outcome favorability. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 558-583. *Brockner, J., Tyler, T. Μ., & Cooper-Schneider, R. (1992). The influence of prior commitment to an institution on reactions to perceived unfair­ ness: The higher they are, the harder they fall. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 241-261. Brockner, J., & Wiesenfeld, B. M. (1996). An integrative framework for explaining reactions to decisions: Interactive effects of outcomes and procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 189-208. *Brockner, J., Wiesenfeld, В. Μ., & Martin, C. L. (1995). Decision frame, procedural justice, and survivors’ reactions to job layoffs. Organiza­ tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63, 59-68. *Brockner, J., Wiesenfeld, В. Μ., Reed, T., Grover, S., & Martin, C. (1993). Interactive effect of job content and context on the reactions of layoff survivors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 187-197. Byrne, Z. S., & Cropanzano, R. S. (2000, April). To which do I attribute this fairness? Differential effects of multi-foci justice on organizational work behaviors. Paper presented at the 15th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, New Orleans, LA. *Cobb, A. T., & Frey, F. Μ. (1996). The effects of leader fairness and pay outcomes on superior/subordinate relations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26. 1401-1426. ♦Cobb, A. T., Vest. Μ., & Hills, F. (1997). Who delivers justice? Source perceptions of procedural fairness. Journal of Applied Social Psychol­ ogy, 27, 1021-1040. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Eribaum. *Colquitt, J. A. (1999). The impact of procedural justice in teams: An analysis of task, team, and member moderators. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing. *Colquitt, J. A. (2000). Justice in teams: An analysis of antecedents and consequences. Unpublished manuscript. *Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386-400. *Colquitt, J. A., & Chertkoff, J. M. (1996, April). Explaining injustice: The interactive effect of explanation and outcome on fairness perceptions and task motivation. Paper presented at the 11th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA. Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 678-707. *Conlon, D. E. (1993). Some tests of the self-interest and group value models of procedural justice: Evidence from an organizational appeal procedure. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 1109-1124. *Conlon, D. E., & Fasolo, P. M. (1990). Influence of speed of third-party intervention and outcome on negotiator and constituent fairness judg­ ments. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 833-846. *Conlon, D. E., & Lynch, P. D. (1997, June). The role of contracts and explanations on justice judgments and agent compensation. Paper pre­ sented at the 10th Annual Conference of the International Association for Conflict Management, Bonn, Germany. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Murray, N. M. (1996). Customer perceptions of corpo­ rate responses to product complaints: The role of explanations. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 1040-1056. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Parks, J. Μ. (1999). Effects of resources and reward allocation rules on justice judgments. Unpublished manuscript. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Ross, W. H. (1993). The effects of partisan third parties on negotiator behavior and outcome perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 280-290. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Ross, W. H. (1997). Appearances do count: The effects of outcomes and explanations on disputant fairness judgments and supervisory evaluations. International Journal of Conflict Manage­ ment, 8, 5-31. Cropanzano, R., & Ambrose, M. L. (2001). Procedural and distributive justice are more similar than you think: A monistic perspective and a research agenda. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 119-151). Lexington, MA: New Lexington Press. Cropanzano, R., & Greenberg, J. (1997). Progress in organizational justice: Tunneling through the maze. In C. Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 317-372). New York: Wiley. ♦Cropanzano, R., & Konovsky, M. A. (1996). Resolving the justice di­ lemma by improving the outcomes: The case of employee drug screen­ ing. Journal of Business and Psychology, II, 239-263. ♦Cropanzano, R., & Prehar, C. A. (1999, April). Using social exchange theory to distinguish procedural from interactional justice. Paper pre­ sented at the 14th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Atlanta, GA. Cudeck, R. (1989). Analysis of correlation matrices using covariance structure models. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 317-327. ♦Dailey, R. C., & Kirk, D. J. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as antecedents of job dissatisfaction and intent to turnover. Human Rela­ tions, 45, 305-317. ♦Daly, J. P. (1995). Explaining changes to employees: The influence of justifications and change outcomes on employees’ fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 31, 415-428. ♦Daly, J. P., & Geyer, P. D. (1994). The role of fairness in implementing large-scale change: Employee evaluations of process and outcome in seven facility relocations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 623­ 638. ♦Davy, J. A., Kinicki, A. J., & Scheck, C. L. (1991). Developing and testing a model of survivor responses to layoffs. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 38, 302-317. Deutsch, Μ. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31, 137-150. ♦DeWitt, R. L., Trevino, L. K., & Mollica, K. A. (1998). The influence of eligibility on employees’ reactions to voluntary workforce reductions. Journal of Management, 24, 593-613. ♦Dittrich, J. E., & Carrell, Μ. R. (1979). Organizational equity perceptions, employee job satisfaction, and department absence and turnover rates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 24, 29-40. ♦Donovan, M. A., Drasgow, F., & Munson, L. J. (1998). The Perceptions of Fair Interpersonal Treatment Scale: Development and validation of a measure of interpersonal treatment in the workplace. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 683-692. ♦Dulebohn, J. H., & Ferris, G. R. (1999). The role of influence tactics in perceptions of performance evaluations’ fairness. Academy of Manage­ ment Journal, 42, 288-303. ♦Dulebohn, J. H., & Martocchio, J. J. (1998). Employee perceptions of the fairness of work group incentive pay plans. Journal of Management, 24, 469-488. ♦Earley, P. C., & Lind, E. A. (1987). Procedural justice and participation in task selection: The role of control in mediating justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1148-1160. ♦Ehlen, C. R., Magner, N. R., & Welker, R. B. (1999). Testing for interactive effects of outcome favourability and procedural fairness on members’ reactions towards a voluntary professional organization. Jour­ nal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 72, 147-161. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 441 ♦Farh, J., Earley, P. C., & Lin, S. (1997). Impetus for action: A cultural analysis of justice and organizational citizenship behavior in Chinese society. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 421-444. Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: Combined impact of “voice” and improvement on experienced inequity Journal of Person­ ality and Social Psychology, 35, 108-119. Folger, R. (1987). Distributive and procedural justice in the workplace. Social Justice Research, 1, 143-159. Folger, R., & Bies, R. J. (1989). Managerial responsibilities and procedural justice. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 2, 79-89. Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Organizational justice and human resource management. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. *Folger, R., & Konovsky, Μ. A. (1989). Effects of procedural and distrib­ utive justice on reactions to pay raise decisions. Academy of Manage­ ment Journal, 32, 115-130. ♦Folger, R„ & Martin, C. (1986). Relative deprivation and referent cog­ nitions: Distributive and procedural justice effects. Journal of Experi­ mental Social Psychology, 22, 531-546. *Folger, R., Rosenfield, D., Grove, J., & Corkran, L. (1979). Effects of “voice” and peer opinions and responses to inequity. Journal of Person­ ality and Social Psychology, 37, 2253-2261. ♦Folger, R., Rosenfield, D., Hays, R., & Grove, R. (1978). Justice vs. justification effects on productivity: Reconciling equity and dissonance findings. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 22, 465-473. ♦Folger, R., & Skarlicki, D. P. (1998). When tough times make tough bosses: Managerial distancing as a function of layoff blame. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 79-87. *Frost, S. E. (Ed.). (1972). Masterworks of philosophy (Vol. 1, pp. 135­ 140). Boston: McGraw-Hill. *Fryxell, G. E., & Gordon, Μ. E. (1989). Workplace justice and job satisfaction as predictors of satisfaction with union and management. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 851-866. *Gellatly, I. R. (1995). Individual and group determinants of employee absenteeism: Test of a causal model. Journal of Organizational Behav­ ior, 16, 469-485. ♦George, J. Μ. (1991). State or trait: Effects of positive mood on prosocial behaviors at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299-307. ♦Giacobbe-Miller, J. (1995). A test of the group values and control models of procedural justice from the competing perspectives of labor and management. Personnel Psychology, 48, 115-142. ♦Giles, W. F., Findley, H. Μ., & Field, H. S. (1997). Procedural fairness in performance appraisal: Beyond the review session. Journal of Business and Psychology, 11, 493-506. ♦Gilliland, S. W. (1994). Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions to a selection system. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 691-701. ♦Gilliland, S. W., & Beckstein, В. A. (1996). Procedural and distributive justice in the editorial review process. Personnel Psychology, 49, 669­ 691. ♦Gilliland, S. W., Benson, L., & Schepers, D. H. (1998). A rejection threshold in justice evaluations: Effects on judgment and decision­ making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 113-131. ♦Gordon, M. E., & Bowlby, R. L. (1988). Propositions about grievance settlements: Finally, consultation with grievants. Personnel Psychol­ ogy, 41, 107-123. ♦Gordon, Μ. E., & Fryxell, G. E. (1989). Voluntariness of association as a moderator of the importance of procedural and distributive justice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 993-1009. Graen, G. B., & Scandura, T. (1987). Toward a psychology of dyadic organizing. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 175-208). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. ♦Greenberg, J. (1987a). Reactions to procedural injustice in payment distributions: Do the means justify the ends? Journal of Applied Psy­ chology, 72, 55-61. Greenberg, J. (1987b). A taxonomy of organizational justice theories. Academy of Management Review, 12, 9-22. Greenberg, J. (1990a). Employee theft as a reaction to underpayment inequity: The hidden cost of pay cuts. Journal of Applied Psychol­ ogy, 75, 561-568. Greenberg, J. (1990b). Organizational justice: Yesterday, today, and to­ morrow. Journal of Management, 16, 399-432. Greenberg, J. (1993a). The intellectual adolescence of organizational jus­ tice: You’ve come a long way, maybe. Social Justice Research, 6, 135-148. Greenberg, J. (1993b). The social side of fairness: Interpersonal and informational classes of organizational justice. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: Approaching fairness in human resource management (pp. 79-103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ♦Greenberg, J. (1993c). Stealing in the name of justice: Informational and interpersonal moderators of theft reactions to underpayment inequity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 81-103. ♦Greenberg, J. (1994). Using socially fair treatment to promote acceptance of a work site smoking ban. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 288­ 297. ♦Grienberger, I. V., Rutte, C. G., & Van Knippenberg, A. F. Μ. (1997). Influence of social comparisons of outcomes and procedures on fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 913-919. ♦Griffeth, R. W., Vecchio, R. P., & Logan, J. W. (1989). Equity theory and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 394-401. ♦Hagedoom, Μ., Buunk, B. P., & Van de Vliert, E. (1998). Opening the black box between justice and reactions to unfavorable outcomes in the workplace. Social Justice Research, 11, 41-57. ♦Hammer, T. H., Landau, J. C., & Stem, R. N. (1981). Absenteeism when workers have a voice: The case of employee ownership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 561-573. ♦Harrison, D. A., McLaughlin, Μ. E., & Coalter, T. Μ. (1996). Context, cognition, and common method variance: Psychometric and verbal pro­ tocol evidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro­ cesses, 68, 246-261. Hom, P. W., Caranikas-Walker, F„ Prussia, G. E., & Griffeth, R. W. (1992). A meta-analytical structural equations analysis of a model of employee turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 890-909. ♦Hom, P. W„ Griffeth, R. W„ & Sellare, C. L. (1984). The validity of Mobley’s (1977) model of employee turnover. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 34, 141-174. Homans, G. (1961). Social behaviour: Its elementary forms. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. ♦Jones, F. F., Scarpello, V., & Bergmann, T. (1999). Pay procedures: What makes them fair? Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol­ ogy, 72, 129-145. ♦Joy, V. L., & Witt, L. A. (1992). Delay of gratification as a moderator of the procedural justice-distributive justice relationship. Group and Orga­ nization Management, 17, 297-308. ♦Kanfer, R., Sawyer, J. E., Earley, P. C., & Lind, E. A. (1987). Fairness and participation in evaluation procedures: Effects on task attitudes and performance. Social Justice Research, I, 235-249. *Karambayya, R., & Brett, J. Μ. (1989). Managers handling disputes: Third-party roles and perceptions of fairness. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 687-704. 442 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG *Keller, T., & Dansereau, F. (1995). Leadership and empowerment: A social exchange perspective. Human Relations, 48, 127-146. *Kidwell, R. E., & Bennett, N. (1994). Employee reactions to electronic control systems: The role of procedural fairness. Group and Organiza­ tion Management, 19, 203-218. *Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1993). Procedural justice, attitudes, and subsidiary top management compliance with multinationals’ corpo­ rate strategic decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 502-526. *Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1996). Procedural justice and manag­ ers’ in-role and extra-role behavior: The case of the multinational. Management Science, 42, 499-515. *King, W. C., Jr., Miles, E. W., & Day, D. D. (1993). A test and refinement of the equity sensitivity construct. Journal of Organizational Behav­ ior, 14, 301-317. ♦Konovsky, Μ. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1991). Perceived fairness of em­ ployee drug testing as a predictor of employee attitudes and job perfor­ mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 698-707. ♦Konovsky, M. A., & Folger, R. (1991). The effects of procedures, social accounts, and benefits level on victims’ layoff reactions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 630—650. *Konovsky, Μ. A., Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1987). Relative effects of procedural and distributive justice on employee attitudes. Represen­ tative Research in Social Psychology, 17, 15-24. *Konovsky, Μ. A., & Pugh, S. D. (1994). Citizenship behavior and social exchange. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 656-669. *Korsgaard, Μ. A., & Roberson, L. (1995). Procedural justice in perfor­ mance evaluation: The role of instrumental and non-instrumental voice in performance appraisal discussions. Journal of Management, 21, 657­ 669. *Korsgaard, M. A., Roberson, L., & Rymph, R. D. (1998). What motivates fairness? The role of subordinate assertive behavior on managers’ inter­ actional fairness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 731-744. *Korsgaard, Μ. A., Schweiger, D. Μ., & Sapienza, H. J. (1995). Building commitment, attachment, and trust in strategic decision-making teams: The role of procedural justice. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 60-84. *Latack, J. C., Josephs, S. L., Roach, B. L., & Levine, M. D. (1987). Carpenter apprentices: Comparison of career transitions for men and women. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 393-400. *LaTour, S. (1978). Determinants of participant and observer satisfaction with adversary and inquisitorial modes of adjudication. Journal of Per­ sonality and Social Psychology, 12, 1531—1545. *Lawson, M. B., & Angle, H. L. (1998). Upon reflection: Commitment, satisfaction, and regret after a corporate relocation. Group and Organi­ zation Management, 23, 289-317. *Lee, C., & Farh, J. L. (1999). The effects of gender in organizational justice perception. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 133-143. *Leung, K., & Li, W. (1990). Psychological mechanisms of process­ control effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 613-620. ♦Leventhal, G. S. (1976). The distribution of rewards and resources in groups and organizations. In L. Berkowitz & W. Walster (Eds.), Ad­ vances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 91-131). New York: Academic Press. Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. Gergen, M. Greenberg, & R. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27-55). New York: Plenum. Leventhal, G. S., Karuža, J., & Fry, W. R. (1980). Beyond fairness: A theory of allocation preferences. In G. Mikula (Ed.), Justice and social interaction (pp. 167-218). New York: Springer-Verlag. *Levy, P. E., & Williams, J. R. (1998). The role of perceived system knowledge in predicting appraisal reactions, job satisfaction, and orga­ nizational commitment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 53-65. ♦Lind, E. A., Kulik, C. T., Ambrose, M. A., & de Vera Park, M. V. (1993). Individual and corporate dispute resolution: Using procedural fairness as a decision heuristic. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 224-251. ♦Lind, E. A., Lissak, R. I., & Conlon, D. F. (1983). Decision control and process control effects on procedural fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 338-350. Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum. ♦Lind, E. A., Tyler, T. R., & Huo, Y. J. (1997). Procedural context and culture: Variation in the antecedents of procedural justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 767-780. ♦Lowe, R. H., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1995). A field study of distributive and procedural justice as predictors of satisfaction and organizational com­ mitment. Journal of Business and Psychology, 10, 99-114. *Macan, T. H., Avedon, M. J., Paese, Μ., & Smith, D. B. (1994). The effects of applicants’ reactions to cognitive ability tests and an assess­ ment center. Personnel Psychology, 47, 715-738. ♦Magner, N. R., Rahman, Μ., & Welker, R. B. (1996). The interactive effect of outcome favorability and procedural justice in work resource allocation on work performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychol­ ogy, 26, 825-842. ♦Mansour-Cole, D. Μ., & Scott, S. G. (1998). Hearing it through the grapevine: The influence of source, leader-relations, and legitimacy on survivors’ fairness perceptions. Personnel Psychology, 51, 25-54. ♦Mark, Μ. Μ. (1985). Expectations, procedural justice, and alternative reactions to being deprived of a desired outcome. Journal of Experi­ mental Social Psychology, 21, 114-137. ♦Martin, C. L., & Bennett, N. (1996). The role of justice judgments in explaining the relationship between job satisfaction and organizational commitment. Group and Organization Management, 21, 84-104. ♦Martin, C. L., Parsons, C. K., & Bennett, N. (1995). The influence of employee involvement program membership during downsizing: Atti­ tudes toward the employer and the union. Journal of Management, 21, 879-890. ♦Martin, J. E., & Peterson, Μ. Μ. (1987). Two-tier wage structures: Implications for equity theory. Academy of Management Journal, 30, 297-315. ♦Martocchia, J. J., & Judge, T. A. (1995). When we don’t see eye to eye: Discrepancies between supervisors and subordinates in absence disci­ plinary procedures. Journal of Management, 21, 251-278. Masterson, S. S., Bartol, К. Μ., & Moye, N. (2000, April). Interactional and procedural justice: Type versus source of fairness. Paper presented at the 15th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Orga­ nizational Psychology, New Orleans, LA. ♦Masterson, S. S., Lewis, K., Goldman, B. Μ., & Taylor, M. S. (2000). Integrating justice and social exchange: The differing effects of fair procedures and treatment on work relationships. Academy of Manage­ ment Journal, 43, 738-748. Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1990). A review and meta-analysis of the antecedents, correlates, and consequences of organizational commit­ ment. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 172-195. ♦McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, P. D. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as predictors of satisfaction with personal and organizational outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 626-637. ♦McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, P. D. (1996). Does having a say matter only if you get your way? Instrumental and value-expressive effects of employee voice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18, 289-303. ♦Miles, J. A., & Klein, H. J. (1998). The fairness of assigning group members to tasks. Group and Organization Management, 23, 71-96. ♦Miller, M. L. (1989). Process and decision control in the work place: Separate effects, independence from distributive justice, and tests of explanatory mechanisms. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 10, 337-354. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 443 *Moorman, R. H. (1991). Relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 845-855. *Moorman, R. H., & Blakely, G. L. (1995). Individualism-collectivism as an individual difference predictor of organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 16, 127-142. *Moorman, R. H., Blakely, G. L., & Niehoff, B. P. (1998). Does perceived organizational support mediate the relationship between procedural jus­ tice and organizational citizenship behavior? Academy of Management Journal, 41, 351-357. *Moorman, R. H., Niehoff, B. P., & Organ, D. W. (1993). Treating employees fairly and organizational citizenship behavior: Sorting the effects of job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and procedural justice. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 6, 209-225. *Mossholder, K. W., Bennett, N., Kemery, E. R., & Wesolowski, M. A. (1998). Relationships between bases of power and work reactions: The mediational role of procedural justice. Journal of Management, 24, 533-552. *Mossholder, K. W., Bennett, N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). A multilevel analysis of procedural justice context. Journal of Organizational Behav­ ior, 19, 131-141. *Moye, N. A., Masterson, S. S., & Bartol, K. M. (1997, August). Differ­ entiating antecedents and consequences of procedural and interactional justice: Empirical evidence in support of separate constructs. Paper presented at the 56th Annual Meeting of the National Academy of Management, Boston, MA. *Newman, K. L. (1993). Procedural justice and ethical decision making. Social Justice Research, 6, 113-134. *Niehoff, B. P., & Moorman, R. H. (1993). Justice as a mediator of the relationship between methods of monitoring and organizational citizen­ ship behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 527-556. *Ogilvie. J. R. (1986). The role of human resource management practices in predicting organizational commitment. Group and Organization Stud­ ies, 11, 335-359. *Olson-Buchanan, J. B. (1996). Voicing discontent: What happens to the grievance filer after the grievance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 52-63. Organ, D. W. (1990). The motivational basis of organizational citizenship behavior. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in orga­ nizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 43-72). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. *Parker, C. P., Baltes, B. B., & Christiansen, N. D. (1997). Support for affirmative action, justice perceptions, and work attitudes: A study of gender and racial-ethnic group differences. Journal of Applied Psychol­ ogy, 82. 376-389. Pillutla, Μ., & Murnighan, J. K. (1999). On fairness and justice in bargaining and in allocation decisions and procedures. Unpublished manuscript. *Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. Μ. (1997). Toward an explanation of applicant reactions: An examination of organizational justice and attri­ bution frameworks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro­ cesses, 72, 308 -335. *Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. M. (1998). Applicants’ reactions to the fairness of selection procedures: The effects of positive rule violations and time of measurement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 3-16. *Ployhart, R. E., Ryan, A. Μ., & Bennett, Μ. (1999). Explanations for selection decisions: Applicants’ reactions to informational and sensitiv­ ity features of explanations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 87-106. Podsakoff, P. Μ., MacKenzie, S. B„ & Bommer, W. H. (1996). Meta­ analysis of the relationships between Kerr and Jermier's substitutes for leadership and employee job attitudes, role perceptions, and perfor- manee. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81. 380-399. *Pond, S. B., III, Nacoste, R. W., Mohr, M. F., & Rodriguez, C. M. (1997). The measurement of organizational citizenship behavior: Are we assum­ ing too much? Journal of Applied Psychology, 27, 1527-1554. *Porter, C. O. L. H., Conlon, D. E„ & Barber, A. E. (1999, August). Effects of justice in salary negotiations on recruitment outcomes: A laboratory experiment. Paper presented at the 58th Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, Chicago, IL. *Rasinski, D. A. (1990). Contextualizing procedural fairness: Factors mediating the effect of process control on leadership evaluation in organizations. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 459-477. Reichers, A. E., & Schneider, B. (1990). Climate and culture: Life cycles of constructs. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture (pp. 5-39). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. *Richard, O. C., & Kirby, S. L. (1998). Women recruits’ perceptions of workforce diversity program selection decisions: A procedural justice examination. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 183-188. Ryan, A. (1993). Justice. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. *Sapienza, H. J., & Korsgaard, M. A. (1996). Procedural justice in entrepreneur-investor relations. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 544-574. *Scandura, T. A. (1997). Mentoring and organizational justice: An empir­ ical investigation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 51, 58-69. *Scarpello, V., & Jones, F. F. (1996). Why justice matters in compensation decision making. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17, 285-299. *Schappe, S. P. (1998). The influence of job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and fairness perceptions on organizational citizenship be­ havior. Journal of Psychology, 132, 277-290. *Schaubroeck, J., May, D. R., & Brown, F. W. (1994). Procedural justice explanations and employee reactions to economic hardship: A field experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 455-460. *Scher, S. J. (1997). Measuring the consequences of injustice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 482-497. *Schmidt, F. L., Hunter, J. E., & Outerbridge, A. N. (1986). Impact of job experience and ability on job knowledge, work sample performance, and supervisory ratings of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychol­ ogy, 71, 432-439. *Schminke, Μ., Ambrose, M. L., & Noel, T. W. (1997). The effect of ethical frameworks on perceptions of organizational justice. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 1190-1207. *Schmitt, Μ. (1996). Individual differences in sensitivity to befallen in­ justice (SB1). Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 3-20. *Shapiro, D. L. (1991). The effects of explanations on negative reactions to deceit. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 614-630. *Shapiro, D. L., & Brett, J. Μ. (1993). Comparing three processes under­ lying judgments of procedural justice: A field study of mediation and arbitration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1167­ 1177. *Shapiro, D. L„ Buttner, E. H., & Barry, B. (1994). Explanations: What factors enhance their perceived adequacy? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58, 346-368. *Skarlicki, D. P., Ellard, J. H., & Kelln, B. R. C. (1998). Third-party perceptions of a layoff: Procedural, derogation, and retributive aspects of justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 119-127. *Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. (1997). Retaliation in the workplace: The roles of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 434 -443. *Skarlicki, D. P., Folger, R., & Tesluk, P. (1999). Personality as a mod­ erator in the relationship between fairness and retaliation. Academy of Management Journal, 42, 100—108. *Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (1996). Increasing citizenship behavior within a labor union: A test of organizational justice theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 161-169. *Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (1997). Leadership training in organi- 444 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG zational justice to increase citizenship behavior within a labor union: A replication. Personnel Psychology, 50, 617-633. *Smith, H. J., & Tyler, T. R. (1996). Justice and power: When will justice concerns encourage the advantaged to support policies which redistrib­ ute economic resources and the disadvantaged to willingly obey the law? European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 171-200. ♦Smith, H. J., Tyler, T. R., Huo, Y. J., Ortiz, D. J., & Lind, E. A. (1998). The self-relevant implications of the group-value model: Group mem­ bership, self-worth, and treatment quality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 470-493. *Smither, J. W., Reilly, R. R., Millsap, R. E., Pearlman, K., & Stoffey, R. W. (1993). Applicant reactions to selection procedures. Personnel Psychology, 46, 49-76. *Spencer, D. G. (1986). Employee voice and employee retention. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 488-502. *Sujak, D. A., Villanova, P., & Daly, J. P. (1995). The effects of drug­ testing program characteristics on applicants’ attitudes toward potential employment. Journal of Psychology, 129, 401-416. ♦Summers, T. P., & DeNisi, A. S. (1990). In search of Adams’ other: Reexamination of referents used in the evaluation of pay. Human Rela­ tions, 43, 497-511. ♦Sweeney, P. D. (1990). Distributive justice and pay satisfaction: A field test of an equity theory prediction. Journal of Business and Psychol­ ogy, 4, 329-341. ♦Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, D. B. (1993). Workers’ evaluation of the “ends” and the “means”: An examination of four models of distributive and procedural justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 23-40. ♦Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, D. B. (1997). Process and outcome: Gender differences in the assessment of justice. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 83-98. ♦Sweeney, P. D., McFarlin, D. B., & Cotton, J. L. (1991). Locus of control as a moderator of the relationship between perceived influence and procedural justice. Human Relations, 44, 333-342. ♦Syroit, J., Lodewijkx, H., Franssen, E., & Gerstel, I. (1993). Organiza­ tional commitment and satisfaction with work among transferred em­ ployees: An application of referent cognitions theory. Social Justice Research, 6, 219-234. ♦Tansky, J. W. (1993). Justice and organizational citizenship behavior: What is the relationship? Employee Responsibilities and Rights Jour­ nal, 6, 195-207. ♦Tata, J., & Bowes-Sperry, L. (1996). Emphasis on distributive, proce­ dural, and interactional justice: Differential perceptions of men and women. Psychological Reports, 79, 1327-1330. ♦Taylor, M. S., Tracy, K. B., Renard, M. K., Harrison, J. K., & Carroll, S. J. (1995). Due process in performance appraisal: A quasi-experiment in procedural justice. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 495-523. ♦Tepper, B. J., Eisenbach, R. A., Kirby, S. L., & Potter, P. W. (1998). Test of a justice based model of subordinates’ resistance to downward influ­ ence attempts. Group and Organization Management, 23, 144-160. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ♦Tremblay, Μ., Sire, B., & Pelchat, A. (1998). A study of the determinants and of the impact of flexibility on employee benefit satisfaction. Human Relations, 51, 667-688. ♦Truxillo, D. Μ., & Bauer, T. N. (1999). Applicant reactions to test score banding in entry-level and promotional contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 322-339. ♦Tyler, T. R. (1987). Conditions leading to value-expressive effects in judgments of procedural justice: A test of four models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 333-344. Tyler, T. R. (1989). The psychology of procedural justice: A test of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 830-838. Tyler, T. R. (1990). Why people obey the law: Procedural justice, legiti­ macy, and compliance. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Tyler, T. R. (1994). Psychological models of the justice motive: Anteced­ ents of distributive and procedural justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 850-863. Tyler, T. R., & Bies, R. J. (1990). Beyond formal procedures: The inter­ personal context of procedural justice. In J. S. Carroll (Ed.), Applied social psychology and organizational settings (pp. 77-98). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ♦Tyler, T. R., & Caine, A. (1981). The influence of outcomes and proce­ dures on satisfaction with formal leaders. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 642-655. ♦Tyler, T. R., & Degoey, P. (1995). Collective restraint in social dilemmas: Procedural justice and social identification effects on support for author­ ities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 482-497. Tyler, T. R„ Degoey, P., & Smith, H. J. (1996). Understanding why the justice of group procedures matters: A test of the psychological dynam­ ics of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­ chology, 70, 913-930. Tyler, T. R., Lind, E. A., Ohbuchi, K. I., Sugawara, I., & Huo, Y. J. (1998). Conflict with outsiders: Disputing within and across boundaries. Per­ sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 137-146. ♦Tyler, T. R., Rasinski, K. A., & McGraw, K. M. (1985). The influence of perceived justice on the endorsement of political leaders. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 700-725. ♦Tyler, T. R., Rasinski, K. A., & Spodick, N. (1985). Influence of voice on satisfaction with leaders: Exploring the meaning of process control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 72-81. ♦Van den Bos, K., Lind, F. A., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). How do I judge my outcome when I do not know the outcome of others? The psychology of the fair process effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1034-1046. ♦Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1996). The consis­ tency rule and the voice effect: The influence of expectations on proce­ dural fairness judgments and performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 411-428. ♦Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). Procedural and distributive justice: What is fair depends more on what comes first than on what comes next. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 95-104. ♦Van den Bos, K., Wilke, H. A. Μ., & Lind, E. A. (1998). When do we need procedural fairness? The role of trust in authority. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1449-1458. ♦Van Dierendonck, D., Schaufelt, W. B., & Buunk, B. P. (1998). The evaluation of an individual burnout intervention program: The role of inequity and social support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 392­ 407. ♦Vecchio, R. P., Griffeth, R. W., & Hom, P. W. (1986). The predictive utility of the vertical dyad linkage approach. Journal of Social Psychol­ ogy, 126, 617-625. ♦Vermunt, R., Blaauw, F., & Lind, E. A. (1998). Fairness evaluations of encounters with police officers and correctional officers. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1107-1124. Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1995). Theory testing: Combining psy­ chometric meta-analysis and structural equations modeling. Personnel Psychology, 48, 865- 885. ♦Wanberg, C. R., Bunce, L. W., & Gavin, M. B. (1999). Perceived fairness of layoffs among individuals who have been laid off: A longitudinal study. Personnel Psychology, 52, 59 - 84. Wanous, J. P., Sullivan, S. F., & Malinak, J. (1989). The role of judgment calls in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 259-264. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 445 *Weaver, G. R., & Conlon, D. E. (1997, August). Explaining broken promises: Timing, justice effects, and behavioral outcomes. Paper pre­ sented at the 57th Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, Boston, MA. *Welbourne, T. Μ., Balkin, D. B., & Gomez-Mejia, L. R. (1995). Gain­ sharing and mutual monitoring: A combined agency-organizational jus­ tice interpretation. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 881-899. ♦Wesolowski, Μ. A., & Mossholder, K. W. (1997). Relational demography in supervisor-subordinate dyads: Impact on subordinate job satisfaction, burnout, and perceived procedural justice. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 351-362. ♦White, Μ. Μ., Tansky, J. A., & Baik, К. (1995). Linking culture and perceptions of justice: A comparison of students in Virginia and South Korea. Psychological Reports, 77, 1103-1112. Whitener, E. Μ. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and credibility intervals in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 315-321. Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and organiza­ tional commitment as predictors of organizational citizenship and in-role behaviors. Journal of Management, 17, 601-617. Received August 23, 1999 Revision received May 12, 2000 Accepted May 15, 2000 ■ Members of Underrepresented Groups: Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript re­ viewers are vital to the publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate more in this process. If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write to Demarie Jackson at the address below. Please note the following important points: • To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective review. • To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical jour­ nals that are most central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission within the context of existing re­ search. • To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information. Please include with your letter your vita. In your letter, please iden­ tify which APA journal(s) you are interested in, and describe your area of exper­ tise. Be as specific as possible. For example, “social psychology” is not suffi­ cient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude change” as well. • Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1-4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript thoroughly. Write to Demarie Jackson, Journals Office, American Psychological Association, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242. Copyright of Journal of Applied Psychology is the property of American Psychological Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Structure Bookmarks Journal of Applied Journal of Applied Journal of Applied Journal of Applied Psychology 2001, 2001, Vol. 86, No. 3, 425-445 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-9010/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0021-9010.86.3.425 0021-9010/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0021-9010.86.3.425 Justice at the Millennium: A Meta-Analytic Review of 25 Years of Organizational Justice Research Justice at the Millennium: A Meta-Analytic Review of 25 Years of Organizational Justice Research Jason A. Colquitt Jason A. Colquitt University of Florida University of Florida Michael J. Wesson and Christopher O. L. H. Porter Michael J. Wesson and Christopher O. L. H. Porter Texas A&M University Texas A&M University Donald E. Conlon Donald E. Conlon Michigan State University Michigan State University K. Yee Ng K. Yee Ng Michigan State University Michigan State University The field of organizational justice continues to be marked by several important research questions, including the size of relationships among justice dimensions, the relative importance of different justice criteria, and the unique effects of justice dimensions on key outcomes. To address such questions, the authors conducted a meta-analytic review of 183 justice studies. The results suggest that although different justice dimensions are moderately to highly related, they contribute incremental variance exp The field of organizational justice continues to be marked by several important research questions, including the size of relationships among justice dimensions, the relative importance of different justice criteria, and the unique effects of justice dimensions on key outcomes. To address such questions, the authors conducted a meta-analytic review of 183 justice studies. The results suggest that although different justice dimensions are moderately to highly related, they contribute incremental variance exp The study of justice or fairness has been a topic of philosophical interest that extends back at least as far as Plato and Socrates (Ryan, 1993). Colloquially, the term justice is used to connote “oughtness” or “righteousness.” Under the purview of ethics, an act can be defined as just through comparison with a prevailing philosophical system. Unfortunately, often there is no agreement on what that philosophical system should be. For example, Aris­totle (reprinted in Frost, 1972) noted that people in differ The study of justice or fairness has been a topic of philosophical interest that extends back at least as far as Plato and Socrates (Ryan, 1993). Colloquially, the term justice is used to connote “oughtness” or “righteousness.” Under the purview of ethics, an act can be defined as just through comparison with a prevailing philosophical system. Unfortunately, often there is no agreement on what that philosophical system should be. For example, Aris­totle (reprinted in Frost, 1972) noted that people in differ In research in the organizational sciences, justice is considered to be socially constructed. That is, an act is defined as just if most individuals perceive it to be so on the basis of empirical research (Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997). Thus, “what is fair” is derived from past research linking objective facets of decision making to subjective perceptions of fairness. In particular, justice in organi­zational settings can be described as focusing on the antecedents and consequences of two types of subjectiv In research in the organizational sciences, justice is considered to be socially constructed. That is, an act is defined as just if most individuals perceive it to be so on the basis of empirical research (Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997). Thus, “what is fair” is derived from past research linking objective facets of decision making to subjective perceptions of fairness. In particular, justice in organi­zational settings can be described as focusing on the antecedents and consequences of two types of subjectiv Jason A. Colquitt, Department of Management, University of Florida; Donald E. Conlon and K. Yee Ng, Department of Management, Michigan State University; Michael J. Wesson and Christopher Jason A. Colquitt, Department of Management, University of Florida; Donald E. Conlon and K. Yee Ng, Department of Management, Michigan State University; Michael J. Wesson and Christopher O. L. H. Porter, Department of Management, Texas A&M University. We thank George Barnekov and Yumi Eto for their assistance in gathering the articles covered in this review. We thank George Barnekov and Yumi Eto for their assistance in gathering the articles covered in this review. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jason A. Colquitt, Warrington College of Business Administration, Department of Management, University of Florida, P.O. Box 117165, Gainesville, Florida 32611-7165. Electronic mail may be sent to Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jason A. Colquitt, Warrington College of Business Administration, Department of Management, University of Florida, P.O. Box 117165, Gainesville, Florida 32611-7165. Electronic mail may be sent to colquitt@ufl.edu colquitt@ufl.edu . distributive justice (Adams, 1965; Deutsch, 1975; Homans, 1961; Leventhal, 1976) and procedural justice (Leventhal, 1980; Lev­enthal, distributive justice (Adams, 1965; Deutsch, 1975; Homans, 1961; Leventhal, 1976) and procedural justice (Leventhal, 1980; Lev­enthal, Karuža, & Fry, 1980; Thibaut & Walker, 1975). Efforts to explain the impact of justice on effective organiza­tional functioning have come under the rubric of organizational justice research (Greenberg, 1987b, 1990b). Greenberg (1990b) described organizational justice as a literature “grown around attempts to describe and explain the role of fairness as a consid­eration in the workplace” (p. 400). This literature includes both field and laboratory research, and organizational justice has been among the most frequently researched topics in industrial- or Efforts to explain the impact of justice on effective organiza­tional functioning have come under the rubric of organizational justice research (Greenberg, 1987b, 1990b). Greenberg (1990b) described organizational justice as a literature “grown around attempts to describe and explain the role of fairness as a consid­eration in the workplace” (p. 400). This literature includes both field and laboratory research, and organizational justice has been among the most frequently researched topics in industrial- or As interest in organizational justice has proliferated, so too have the theoretical approaches used to study it, particularly in relation to procedural justice. These approaches each propose a different way of conceptualizing justice, from the provision of process control (Thibaut & Walker, 1975) to a focus on consistency (Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal As interest in organizational justice has proliferated, so too have the theoretical approaches used to study it, particularly in relation to procedural justice. These approaches each propose a different way of conceptualizing justice, from the provision of process control (Thibaut & Walker, 1975) to a focus on consistency (Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal et al., 1980) and an examination of interpersonal treatment (Bies & Moag, 1986). In addition, a large number of studies have sought to link justice perceptions to a variety of organizational outcomes, including job satisfaction, or­ganizational commitment, withdrawal, and organizational citizen­ship behavior. Because of this diversity in theoretical approach and construct focus, organizational justice is a field in need of integration. There have been a number of narrative reviews that have sought to achieve such integration (e.g., Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997; Folger & Cropanzano, 1998; Lind & Tyler, 1988; Greenberg, 1987b, 1990b). However, important questions remain, including the following: How highly related are the different dimensions of organizational justice, and can they be empirically distinguished Because of this diversity in theoretical approach and construct focus, organizational justice is a field in need of integration. There have been a number of narrative reviews that have sought to achieve such integration (e.g., Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997; Folger & Cropanzano, 1998; Lind & Tyler, 1988; Greenberg, 1987b, 1990b). However, important questions remain, including the following: How highly related are the different dimensions of organizational justice, and can they be empirically distinguished 425 425 425 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG 426 426 from one another? Have the different ways of conceptualizing justice improved our ability to create perceptions of fairness? and What are the relationships between different organizational justice dimensions and important outcomes relevant to organizations? The first question deals with issues of construct discrimination (i.e., to what extent are constructs distinct from one another?). The second question deals with what Greenberg (1987b) called proactive re­search (i.e., research devoted to creating percep from one another? Have the different ways of conceptualizing justice improved our ability to create perceptions of fairness? and What are the relationships between different organizational justice dimensions and important outcomes relevant to organizations? The first question deals with issues of construct discrimination (i.e., to what extent are constructs distinct from one another?). The second question deals with what Greenberg (1987b) called proactive re­search (i.e., research devoted to creating percep The purpose of this study was to conduct a comprehensive, meta-analytic review of the existing literature on organizational justice. To our knowledge, a meta-analysis has never been con­ducted in the organizational justice literature, even though meta­analysis has several important strengths relative to traditional narrative reviews (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). First, meta-analysis is a more powerful summarizing tool because it yields a quantita­tive population value for a relationship of interest. Second, met The purpose of this study was to conduct a comprehensive, meta-analytic review of the existing literature on organizational justice. To our knowledge, a meta-analysis has never been con­ducted in the organizational justice literature, even though meta­analysis has several important strengths relative to traditional narrative reviews (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). First, meta-analysis is a more powerful summarizing tool because it yields a quantita­tive population value for a relationship of interest. Second, met We conducted meta-analyses on all articles in the organizational justice literature published since 1975. We chose 1975 as our starting date because this is when Thibaut and Walker introduced the procedural justice construct, which allowed for the compara­tive study of the influence of multiple dimensions of justice. That year also marks, approximately, the time when justice researchers began integrating fairness concerns with outcomes relevant to organizations (e.g., job satisfaction and organizational com We conducted meta-analyses on all articles in the organizational justice literature published since 1975. We chose 1975 as our starting date because this is when Thibaut and Walker introduced the procedural justice construct, which allowed for the compara­tive study of the influence of multiple dimensions of justice. That year also marks, approximately, the time when justice researchers began integrating fairness concerns with outcomes relevant to organizations (e.g., job satisfaction and organizational com A Brief Review of the Organizational Justice Literature A Brief Review of the Organizational Justice Literature Introduction of Distributive Justice Introduction of Distributive Justice Before 1975, the study of justice was primarily concerned with distributive justice. Much of this research was derived from initial work conducted by Adams (1965), who used a social exchange theory framework to evaluate fairness. According to Adams, what people were concerned about was not the absolute level of out­comes per se but whether those outcomes were fair. Adams sug­gested that one way to determine whether an outcome was fair was to calculate the ratio of one’s contributions or “inputs” (e.g., educ Before 1975, the study of justice was primarily concerned with distributive justice. Much of this research was derived from initial work conducted by Adams (1965), who used a social exchange theory framework to evaluate fairness. According to Adams, what people were concerned about was not the absolute level of out­comes per se but whether those outcomes were fair. Adams sug­gested that one way to determine whether an outcome was fair was to calculate the ratio of one’s contributions or “inputs” (e.g., educ theory an “objective” component, he was clear that this process was completely subjective. theory an “objective” component, he was clear that this process was completely subjective. Whereas Adams’s theory advocated the use of an equity rule to determine fairness, several other allocation rules have also been identified, such as equality and need (e.g., Leventhal, 1976). Studies have shown that different contexts (e.g., work vs. family), different organizational goals (e.g., group harmony vs. productiv­ity), and different personal motives (e.g., self-interest motives vs. altruistic motives) can activate the use or primacy of certain allocation rules (Deutsch, 1975). Nevertheless, all of Whereas Adams’s theory advocated the use of an equity rule to determine fairness, several other allocation rules have also been identified, such as equality and need (e.g., Leventhal, 1976). Studies have shown that different contexts (e.g., work vs. family), different organizational goals (e.g., group harmony vs. productiv­ity), and different personal motives (e.g., self-interest motives vs. altruistic motives) can activate the use or primacy of certain allocation rules (Deutsch, 1975). Nevertheless, all of Introduction of Procedural Justice Introduction of Procedural Justice With the publication of their book summarizing disputant reac­tions to legal procedures, Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the study of process to the literature on justice. Thibaut and Walker (1975) viewed third-party dispute resolution procedures such as mediation and arbitration as having both a process stage and a decision stage. They referred to the amount of influence disputants had in each stage as evidence of process control and decision control, respectively. Their research suggested that disput With the publication of their book summarizing disputant reac­tions to legal procedures, Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the study of process to the literature on justice. Thibaut and Walker (1975) viewed third-party dispute resolution procedures such as mediation and arbitration as having both a process stage and a decision stage. They referred to the amount of influence disputants had in each stage as evidence of process control and decision control, respectively. Their research suggested that disput Although Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the concept of procedural justice, their work focused primarily on disputant re­actions to legal procedures. Although a focus on justice and law continues to be of interest to scholars (e.g., Tyler, 1990), Leventhal and colleagues can be credited for extending the notion of proce­dural justice into nonlegal contexts such as organizational settings (Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal Although Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the concept of procedural justice, their work focused primarily on disputant re­actions to legal procedures. Although a focus on justice and law continues to be of interest to scholars (e.g., Tyler, 1990), Leventhal and colleagues can be credited for extending the notion of proce­dural justice into nonlegal contexts such as organizational settings (Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal et al., 1980). In doing so, Leventhal and colleagues also broadened the list of determinants of proce­dural justice far beyond the concept of process control. Leventhal’s theory of procedural justice judgments focused on six criteria that a procedure should meet if it is to be perceived as fair. Procedures should (a) be applied consistently across people and across time, (b) be free from bias (e.g., ensuring that a third party has no vested interest in a particular settlement), (c) ensure that accurate infor­m Introduction of Interactional Justice Introduction of Interactional Justice Bies Bies and Moag (1986) introduced the most recent advance in the justice literature by focusing attention on the importance of the quality of the interpersonal treatment people receive when proce­dures are implemented. Bies and Moag (1986) referred to these META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 427 427 aspects of justice as “interactional justice.” More recently, inter­actional justice has come to be seen as consisting of two specific types of interpersonal treatment (e.g., Greenberg, 1990a, 1993b). The first, labeled interpersonal justice, reflects the degree to which people are treated with politeness, dignity, and respect by author­ities or third parties involved in executing procedures or determin­ing outcomes. The second, labeled informational justice, focuses on the explanations provided to people t aspects of justice as “interactional justice.” More recently, inter­actional justice has come to be seen as consisting of two specific types of interpersonal treatment (e.g., Greenberg, 1990a, 1993b). The first, labeled interpersonal justice, reflects the degree to which people are treated with politeness, dignity, and respect by author­ities or third parties involved in executing procedures or determin­ing outcomes. The second, labeled informational justice, focuses on the explanations provided to people t Questions Permeating the Organizational Justice Literature Questions Permeating the Organizational Justice Literature The proliferation of studies on organizational justice has cer­tainly enhanced the visibility of fairness concerns, but the large number of studies and the differing theoretical perspectives raise the concern that justice scholars may be “losing the forest for the trees.” In other words, despite the large accumulation of findings, many central questions remain either unaddressed or unclear. Until such questions are addressed, theory development in the organi­zational justice literature will continue to be h The proliferation of studies on organizational justice has cer­tainly enhanced the visibility of fairness concerns, but the large number of studies and the differing theoretical perspectives raise the concern that justice scholars may be “losing the forest for the trees.” In other words, despite the large accumulation of findings, many central questions remain either unaddressed or unclear. Until such questions are addressed, theory development in the organi­zational justice literature will continue to be h Construct Discrimination Questions Construct Discrimination Questions Perhaps the oldest debate in the justice literature concerns the independence of procedural and distributive justice. Some studies have revealed extremely high correlations between the two justice dimensions, suggesting that they may not be distinct in the minds of many people (Folger, 1987). For example, Sweeney and Mc­Farlin (1997) found an uncorrected correlation of .72 between procedural and distributive justice in a study of attitudes among federal employees. Welbourne, Perhaps the oldest debate in the justice literature concerns the independence of procedural and distributive justice. Some studies have revealed extremely high correlations between the two justice dimensions, suggesting that they may not be distinct in the minds of many people (Folger, 1987). For example, Sweeney and Mc­Farlin (1997) found an uncorrected correlation of .72 between procedural and distributive justice in a study of attitudes among federal employees. Welbourne, Balkin, and Gomez-Mejia (1995) found an uncorrected correlation of .74 in a study of employees in two different companies, one a high-technology firm and the other a consumer products firm. Possibly as a result of such findings, Martocchio and Judge (1995) conducted a study of disciplinary decisions in which no effort was made to separate procedural and distributive justice. Rather, the authors examined the effects of “organizational justice.” Such high correlations are congruent with theoretical arguments made by Cropanzano and Ambrose (2001). These authors have argued that the procedural justice-distributive justice distinction, although necessary and valuable, may sometimes be overempha­sized. Their “monistic perspective” notes that procedural evalua­tions are based in large part on outcomes attained (Thibaut & Walker, 1975) and that the same event can be seen as a process in one context and an outcome in another. For example, reorganizing a p Such high correlations are congruent with theoretical arguments made by Cropanzano and Ambrose (2001). These authors have argued that the procedural justice-distributive justice distinction, although necessary and valuable, may sometimes be overempha­sized. Their “monistic perspective” notes that procedural evalua­tions are based in large part on outcomes attained (Thibaut & Walker, 1975) and that the same event can be seen as a process in one context and an outcome in another. For example, reorganizing a p The construct discrimination concern applies to an even greater degree to procedural and interactional justice. The construct discrimination concern applies to an even greater degree to procedural and interactional justice. Bies and Moag (1986) originally declared interactional justice to be a third type of justice. They argued that people draw on interactional justice perceptions when deciding how to react to authority figures (i.e., bosses and supervisors), whereas procedural justice perceptions are used to decide how to react to the overall organization. How­ever, are used to decide how to react to the overall organization. How­ever, Bies retracted the position that interactional justice was a third type of justice in a subsequent review (Tyler & Bies, 1990). The author’s retraction of his earlier stance has become widely held, as one recent narrative review treated interactional justice as a social form of procedural justice (Cropanzano & Greenberg, 1997). In keeping with this view, many researchers have opera­tionalized procedural justice by measuring process control or Lev­enthal criteria, along with interactional justice, in one combined scale (e.g., Brockner, Siegel, Daly, & Martin, 1997; Brockner, Wiesenfeld, & Martin, 1995; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). This practice seems to suggest that the inter­personal implementation of procedures need not (or cannot) be separated from their structural aspects. However, other research has renewed the debate surrounding the distinctiveness of procedural and interactional justice. Studies that have examined the two constructs separately have shown that they have different correlates or independent effects, or both (e.g., Barling & Phillips, 1993; However, other research has renewed the debate surrounding the distinctiveness of procedural and interactional justice. Studies that have examined the two constructs separately have shown that they have different correlates or independent effects, or both (e.g., Barling & Phillips, 1993; Blader & Tyler, 2000; Cropanzano & Prehar, 1999; Masterson, Lewis, Goldman, & Taylor, 2000; Moye, Masterson, & Bartol, 1997; Skarlicki & Folger, 1997). Blader and Tyler (2000), in a survey of 404 U.S. workers, found that system­originating procedural factors and leader-originating procedural factors remained distinct in a confirmatory factor analysis. Master­ son, Lewis, et al. (2000) drew on social exchange theory to show that procedural and interactional justice affected other variables through different intervening mechanisms. Specifically, proce­dural justice affected other variables by altering perceived organi­zational support perceptions; interactional justice affected other variables by altering leader-member exchange perceptions (Graen & Scandura, 1987). Even assuming that interactional justice can and should be distinguished from procedural justice, another question is whether the interpersonal and informational facets of the construct merit conceptual separation. Greenberg (1993b) suggested that interper­sonal and informational justice should be separated because they are logically distinct and have been shown to have independent effects (e.g., Greenberg, 1993c, 1994). Interpersonal justice acts primarily to alter reactions to decision outcomes, because s Even assuming that interactional justice can and should be distinguished from procedural justice, another question is whether the interpersonal and informational facets of the construct merit conceptual separation. Greenberg (1993b) suggested that interper­sonal and informational justice should be separated because they are logically distinct and have been shown to have independent effects (e.g., Greenberg, 1993c, 1994). Interpersonal justice acts primarily to alter reactions to decision outcomes, because s Some recent work has attempted to address the construct dis­crimination questions raised earlier. Colquitt (2001) developed measures of distributive, procedural, informational, and interper­sonal justice based on the seminal introductions of each construct Some recent work has attempted to address the construct dis­crimination questions raised earlier. Colquitt (2001) developed measures of distributive, procedural, informational, and interper­sonal justice based on the seminal introductions of each construct (Bies & Moag, 1986; Leventhal, 1976, 1980; Leventhal et al., 1980; Thibaut & Walker, 1975) and validated them in both a university and a field setting. Colquitt found that a four-factor confirmatory model provided the best fit to the data and further showed that the four justice dimensions predicted different out­comes. Thus, although some progress is being made, the literature on organizational justice is still marked by a debate over whether the domain includes one, two, three, or four dimensions of justice. This is the subject of the first research question a 428 428 428 428 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG Research Question 1: How highly related are the different dimensions of organizational justice, and can they be empirically distinguished from one another? Research Question 1: How highly related are the different dimensions of organizational justice, and can they be empirically distinguished from one another? Proactive Research Questions Proactive Research Questions The history of research devoted to understanding what promotes perceptions of fairness has been marked by increasing complexity in operationalization as new and different conceptualizations have been introduced over the years. Thibaut and Walker (1975) ini­tially assumed that perceptions of procedural fairness were driven by process control. Leventhal and colleagues (Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal The history of research devoted to understanding what promotes perceptions of fairness has been marked by increasing complexity in operationalization as new and different conceptualizations have been introduced over the years. Thibaut and Walker (1975) ini­tially assumed that perceptions of procedural fairness were driven by process control. Leventhal and colleagues (Leventhal, 1980; Leventhal et al., 1980), on the other hand, eschewed the parsimo­nious approach offered by Thibaut and Walker and instead argued that fairness perceptions were created by adherence to six different criteria. Bies and Moag (1986) further suggested that fairness perceptions were created by the proper enactment of procedures in terms of interpersonal and informational justice. Such additional complexity is warranted if new approaches contribute incrementally to our understanding of how to foster perceptions of procedural fairness. Unfortunately, at present we do not actually know whether such incremental contributions exist. This is because so many studies of procedural justice collapse process control, Leventhal criteria, and interpersonal and informa­tional justice into a single variable (e.g., Brockner Such additional complexity is warranted if new approaches contribute incrementally to our understanding of how to foster perceptions of procedural fairness. Unfortunately, at present we do not actually know whether such incremental contributions exist. This is because so many studies of procedural justice collapse process control, Leventhal criteria, and interpersonal and informa­tional justice into a single variable (e.g., Brockner et al., 1995, 1997; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). This makes it impossible to gauge the relative influence of each element on procedural fairness perceptions. Still other studies assess only one conceptualization of procedural justice, again making it im­possible to assess the merits of different approaches. Moreover, it is well accepted that people judge procedures as more fair when they result in fair or favorable outcomes (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Thibaut & Walker, 1975). Despite this, many studies of procedural justice do not also examine distributive justice. Other studies do examine distributive justice but use it only in analyses separate from procedural justice. As a result, we cannot even be sure that process control, Leventhal criteria, and so forth explain variance in procedural fairness perceptions beyon Moreover, it is well accepted that people judge procedures as more fair when they result in fair or favorable outcomes (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Thibaut & Walker, 1975). Despite this, many studies of procedural justice do not also examine distributive justice. Other studies do examine distributive justice but use it only in analyses separate from procedural justice. As a result, we cannot even be sure that process control, Leventhal criteria, and so forth explain variance in procedural fairness perceptions beyon Thus, in terms of creating procedural fairness perceptions, it is unclear to what extent new conceptualizations of procedural jus­tice (including the relatively recent introductions of interpersonal and informational justice) have contributed to our understanding. Nor is it clear how important they are beyond the fairness of outcome distributions. This is the subject of our next two research questions. Thus, in terms of creating procedural fairness perceptions, it is unclear to what extent new conceptualizations of procedural jus­tice (including the relatively recent introductions of interpersonal and informational justice) have contributed to our understanding. Nor is it clear how important they are beyond the fairness of outcome distributions. This is the subject of our next two research questions. Research Question 2: Have the additional conceptualizations of pro­cedural justice developed over time (including interpersonal and in­formational justice) contributed incremental variance explained in procedural fairness perceptions? Research Question 2: Have the additional conceptualizations of pro­cedural justice developed over time (including interpersonal and in­formational justice) contributed incremental variance explained in procedural fairness perceptions? Research Question 3: Do those conceptualizations contribute incre­mental variance explained in procedural fairness perceptions beyond the effects of distributive justice? Research Question 3: Do those conceptualizations contribute incre­mental variance explained in procedural fairness perceptions beyond the effects of distributive justice? Reactive Research Questions Reactive Research Questions Of course, one of the reasons scholars study justice is the belief that enhanced fairness perceptions can improve outcomes relevant to organizations (e.g., organizational commitment, job satisfaction, and performance). One goal of our meta-analytic review is to help develop some consensus regarding the relationships between di­mensions of justice and key outcomes. In particular, we evaluate the predictive ability of three different models that attempt to explain relationships between justice dimensions and Of course, one of the reasons scholars study justice is the belief that enhanced fairness perceptions can improve outcomes relevant to organizations (e.g., organizational commitment, job satisfaction, and performance). One goal of our meta-analytic review is to help develop some consensus regarding the relationships between di­mensions of justice and key outcomes. In particular, we evaluate the predictive ability of three different models that attempt to explain relationships between justice dimensions and Bies and Moag (1986). The relative predictive power of procedural and distributive justice has been of long-standing concern in the justice literature. An interesting aspect of Leventhal’s (1980) work is that he ex­plicitly considered the impact of both procedural and distributive justice and argued that distributive justice is generally more salient than procedural justice. He further argued that distributive justice judgments are likely to be more influential than procedural justice judgments in determining “overall fairness j The relative predictive power of procedural and distributive justice has been of long-standing concern in the justice literature. An interesting aspect of Leventhal’s (1980) work is that he ex­plicitly considered the impact of both procedural and distributive justice and argued that distributive justice is generally more salient than procedural justice. He further argued that distributive justice judgments are likely to be more influential than procedural justice judgments in determining “overall fairness j However, some prior research and theory have questioned this thesis. In perhaps the first empirical study to explicitly link mul­tiple dimensions of justice to organizational outcomes, Alexander and However, some prior research and theory have questioned this thesis. In perhaps the first empirical study to explicitly link mul­tiple dimensions of justice to organizational outcomes, Alexander and Ruderman (1987) surveyed more than 2,000 federal employ­ees, measuring procedural and distributive justice along with six outcomes. Their regression analyses showed that justice measures affected five of six organizational outcomes and that, for four of these five outcomes, procedural justice had stronger relationships than distributive justice. More recently, scholars have argued that distributive justice is likely to exert greater influence on more specific, person- referenced outcomes such as satisfaction with a pay raise or performance evaluation. In contrast, procedural justice is likely to exert greater influence on more general evaluations of systems and authorities (Greenberg, 1990b; Lind & Tyler, 1988). Consistent with this prediction, McFarlin and Sweeney (1992) found that distributive justice was a more important predictor of what they t More recently, scholars have argued that distributive justice is likely to exert greater influence on more specific, person- referenced outcomes such as satisfaction with a pay raise or performance evaluation. In contrast, procedural justice is likely to exert greater influence on more general evaluations of systems and authorities (Greenberg, 1990b; Lind & Tyler, 1988). Consistent with this prediction, McFarlin and Sweeney (1992) found that distributive justice was a more important predictor of what they t Sweeney and McFarlin (1993) conducted perhaps the most comprehensive test of this notion. They contrasted four models expressing the relationship between procedural and distributive justice and two outcomes (pay satisfaction and organizational commitment). The idea that procedural justice predicts more system-referenced outcomes and distributive justice predicts more person-referenced outcomes was termed in their article the “two- factor model.” They found that the two-factor model fit their data better tha Sweeney and McFarlin (1993) conducted perhaps the most comprehensive test of this notion. They contrasted four models expressing the relationship between procedural and distributive justice and two outcomes (pay satisfaction and organizational commitment). The idea that procedural justice predicts more system-referenced outcomes and distributive justice predicts more person-referenced outcomes was termed in their article the “two- factor model.” They found that the two-factor model fit their data better tha META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 429 429 of these studies, we examine the two-factor model’s prediction of differing effects for procedural and distributive justice. of these studies, we examine the two-factor model’s prediction of differing effects for procedural and distributive justice. Finally, Finally, Bies and Moag’s (1986) focus on interpersonal treat­ment suggests a third approach to explaining organizational out­comes, one that explicitly examines the role of interpersonal and informational justice in addition to procedural justice. Recall that Bies and Moag (1986) originally argued that individuals draw on interpersonal and informational justice perceptions when deciding how to react to authority figures (i.e., bosses and supervisors) and draw on procedural justice perceptions when deciding how to react to the overall organization. Building on this work, Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000) drew on social exchange theory (Blau, 1964) and reasoned that individuals in organizations were involved in two types of exchange relationships: exchanges with their imme­diate supervisor and exchanges with the larger organization. In a field study, the authors showed that interactional justice predicted supervisor-referenced outcomes (e.g., citizenship behaviors di­rected at supervisor and supervisor rating of performance), whereas procedural justice predicted organization-referenced out­comes (e.g. The distributive dominance model, the two-factor model, and the agent-system model are explored in the following set of research questions. The distributive dominance model, the two-factor model, and the agent-system model are explored in the following set of research questions. Research Question 4: What are the relationships between distributive justice and important organizational outcomes? Research Question 4: What are the relationships between distributive justice and important organizational outcomes? Research Question 5: What are the relationships between procedural justice and important organizational outcomes? Research Question 5: What are the relationships between procedural justice and important organizational outcomes? Research Question 6: What are the relationships between interper­sonal and informational justice and important organizational outcomes? Research Question 6: What are the relationships between interper­sonal and informational justice and important organizational outcomes? Research Question 7: If considered simultaneously, what are the unique effects of these justice dimensions on key outcomes, and which of the three reactive models receives the most support? Research Question 7: If considered simultaneously, what are the unique effects of these justice dimensions on key outcomes, and which of the three reactive models receives the most support? Our review focuses on several different outcomes representing those most commonly examined in the organizational justice lit­erature. In the following sections, we briefly review each type of outcome. Our review focuses on several different outcomes representing those most commonly examined in the organizational justice lit­erature. In the following sections, we briefly review each type of outcome. Outcome satisfaction. Many justice studies have measured satisfaction with the outcomes of a decision-making process, such as pay, promotions, and performance evaluations. Given the logic presented earlier, we expect that distributive justice judgments will be a better predictor of outcome satisfaction than will procedural justice or interpersonal and informational justice. This pattern has been empirically supported through the use of pay satisfaction and satisfaction with job restructuring (e.g., Folger & Outcome satisfaction. Many justice studies have measured satisfaction with the outcomes of a decision-making process, such as pay, promotions, and performance evaluations. Given the logic presented earlier, we expect that distributive justice judgments will be a better predictor of outcome satisfaction than will procedural justice or interpersonal and informational justice. This pattern has been empirically supported through the use of pay satisfaction and satisfaction with job restructuring (e.g., Folger & Job satisfaction. Many studies also ask about employees’ sat­isfaction with their jobs in general. McFarlin and Sweeney (1992) showed that distributive justice was a more powerful predictor of job satisfaction than was procedural justice. However, this does not seem to fit the two-factor theory argument that procedural Job satisfaction. Many studies also ask about employees’ sat­isfaction with their jobs in general. McFarlin and Sweeney (1992) showed that distributive justice was a more powerful predictor of job satisfaction than was procedural justice. However, this does not seem to fit the two-factor theory argument that procedural justice predicts system-referenced outcomes, whereas distributive justice predicts person-referenced outcomes. Job satisfaction is a more general, multifaceted, and global response than is outcome satisfaction. Consistent with this reasoning, other studies have shown high correlations between procedural justice and job satis­faction (e.g., Mossholder, Bennett, & Martin, 1998; Wesolowski & Mossholder, 1997). In addition, Masterson, Lewis, justice predicts system-referenced outcomes, whereas distributive justice predicts person-referenced outcomes. Job satisfaction is a more general, multifaceted, and global response than is outcome satisfaction. Consistent with this reasoning, other studies have shown high correlations between procedural justice and job satis­faction (e.g., Mossholder, Bennett, & Martin, 1998; Wesolowski & Mossholder, 1997). In addition, Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000) showed procedural justice to be a stronger predictor of job satis­faction than interactional justice, although both had significant independent effects. These results are consistent with the two- factor model and the agent-system model. Organizational commitment. Organizational commitment rep­resents a global, systemic reaction that people have to the company for which they work. Most measures of organizational commit­ment assess affective commitment, the degree to which employees identify with the company and make the company’s goals their own (Allen & Meyer, 1990). Prior work by Tyler (e.g., Tyler, 1990) argues that procedural justice has stronger relationships with support for institutions than does distributive justice. This is also co Organizational commitment. Organizational commitment rep­resents a global, systemic reaction that people have to the company for which they work. Most measures of organizational commit­ment assess affective commitment, the degree to which employees identify with the company and make the company’s goals their own (Allen & Meyer, 1990). Prior work by Tyler (e.g., Tyler, 1990) argues that procedural justice has stronger relationships with support for institutions than does distributive justice. This is also co et al., 2000). Trust. Trust has recently emerged as a popular topic in orga­nizational research (as evidenced by the 1998 Academy of Man­agement Review special issue devoted to the topic). Tyler (1989) argued that trust in decision makers or authorities is important because these people typically have considerable discretion in terms of allocating rewards and resources. Whereas Tyler (1989) initially conceptualized trust in relation to a third party or an authority, Folger and Cropanzano (1998) made the point that trust r Trust. Trust has recently emerged as a popular topic in orga­nizational research (as evidenced by the 1998 Academy of Man­agement Review special issue devoted to the topic). Tyler (1989) argued that trust in decision makers or authorities is important because these people typically have considerable discretion in terms of allocating rewards and resources. Whereas Tyler (1989) initially conceptualized trust in relation to a third party or an authority, Folger and Cropanzano (1998) made the point that trust r Ruder­ man, 1987; Konovsky & Pugh, 1994). However, given that trust is usually referenced to a particular person, the agent-system model would predict that interpersonal and informational justice are even better predictors of this outcome than procedural justice. Evaluation of authority. A number of studies of third-party dispute resolution procedures have asked disputants to make eval­uations of the third party (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Still other work in organizations asks respondents to rate the acceptability of their supervisors or management in more general terms. Much of the research on evaluation of authorities comes from work merging psychology and political science (e.g., Tyler, 1990). Tyler’s (1990) work, along with the two-factor model, would suggest that we Evaluation of authority. A number of studies of third-party dispute resolution procedures have asked disputants to make eval­uations of the third party (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Still other work in organizations asks respondents to rate the acceptability of their supervisors or management in more general terms. Much of the research on evaluation of authorities comes from work merging psychology and political science (e.g., Tyler, 1990). Tyler’s (1990) work, along with the two-factor model, would suggest that we 430 430 430 430 COLQUITT. CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT. CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG and refuted in multiple studies (e.g., Conlon, 1993; Taylor, Tracy, Renard, Harrison, & Carroll, 1995). In addition, the agent-system model would predict that interpersonal and informational justice are better predictors of evaluation of authority in cases in which the authority in question is one’s leader as opposed to management in general. For this reason, our examination of the reactive models distinguishes between agent-referenced evaluations of authority (e.g., focusing on one’s supervisor) and system and refuted in multiple studies (e.g., Conlon, 1993; Taylor, Tracy, Renard, Harrison, & Carroll, 1995). In addition, the agent-system model would predict that interpersonal and informational justice are better predictors of evaluation of authority in cases in which the authority in question is one’s leader as opposed to management in general. For this reason, our examination of the reactive models distinguishes between agent-referenced evaluations of authority (e.g., focusing on one’s supervisor) and system Organizational citizenship behaviors (OCBs). Organ (1990) defined OCBs as behaviors that are discretionary and not explicitly rewarded but that can help improve organizational functioning. Organ (1990) posited that OCBs are driven largely by fairness perceptions. He suggested that people in organizations assume, at the outset, a social exchange relationship. This expectation con­tinues until unfairness is evidenced, at which time the relationship is reinterpreted as economic rather than social. Research on Organizational citizenship behaviors (OCBs). Organ (1990) defined OCBs as behaviors that are discretionary and not explicitly rewarded but that can help improve organizational functioning. Organ (1990) posited that OCBs are driven largely by fairness perceptions. He suggested that people in organizations assume, at the outset, a social exchange relationship. This expectation con­tinues until unfairness is evidenced, at which time the relationship is reinterpreted as economic rather than social. Research on et al. (2000). Thus, our examination of the reactive models distinguishes between agent- referenced OCBs and system-referenced OCBs. Following Wil­liams and Anderson (1991), we refer to the former as individual OCBs (OCBIs) and the latter as organization OCBs (OCBOs). Withdrawal. Behaviors and behavioral intentions such as ab­senteeism, turnover, and neglect are often subsumed under the heading of job withdrawal. Although withdrawal is a relatively common outcome in the justice literature, it has not been examined in the context of the two-factor model. Withdrawal can occur as a result of a thorough, reasoned evaluation of the organization as a system or on a more “spur of the moment” basis in reaction to an unsatisfactory outcome or poor interpersonal treatment by an au Withdrawal. Behaviors and behavioral intentions such as ab­senteeism, turnover, and neglect are often subsumed under the heading of job withdrawal. Although withdrawal is a relatively common outcome in the justice literature, it has not been examined in the context of the two-factor model. Withdrawal can occur as a result of a thorough, reasoned evaluation of the organization as a system or on a more “spur of the moment” basis in reaction to an unsatisfactory outcome or poor interpersonal treatment by an au Sellare, 1984) and other studies revealing effects for procedural justice (e.g., Dailey & Kirk, 1992). Moreover, Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000) showed that procedural justice had more of an impact on withdrawal than interactional justice. Thus, past research has, at various times, supported the distributive dominance model, the two-factor model, and the agent-system model. Negative reactions. Some recent justice research has looked at the relationship between perceived unfairness and a variety of negative reactions, such as employee theft (e.g., Greenberg, 1990a, 1993c) and organizational retaliatory behaviors (ORBs; Skarlicki & Folger, 1997; Skarlicki, Folger, & Tesluk, 1999). As with Negative reactions. Some recent justice research has looked at the relationship between perceived unfairness and a variety of negative reactions, such as employee theft (e.g., Greenberg, 1990a, 1993c) and organizational retaliatory behaviors (ORBs; Skarlicki & Folger, 1997; Skarlicki, Folger, & Tesluk, 1999). As with withdrawal, negative reactions have not been examined from the standpoint of the two-factor model. Whereas negative reactions can occur because of purely cognitive evaluations of the merits of the organization as a whole or as strong emotional reactions to one’s own treatment, reactions such as theft and ORBs clearly damage the larger organizational system. However, Skarlicki and Folger (1997) found that ORBs had approximately equal correla­tions with distributive, procedural, and interactional justice, wit withdrawal, negative reactions have not been examined from the standpoint of the two-factor model. Whereas negative reactions can occur because of purely cognitive evaluations of the merits of the organization as a whole or as strong emotional reactions to one’s own treatment, reactions such as theft and ORBs clearly damage the larger organizational system. However, Skarlicki and Folger (1997) found that ORBs had approximately equal correla­tions with distributive, procedural, and interactional justice, wit Performance. Perhaps the most unclear of all relationships in the justice literature is the relationship between procedural justice and performance. For example, Earley and Lind (1987) found a relationship between procedural fairness judgments and perfor­mance in a laboratory study but not in a field study. Kanfer, Sawyer, Earley, and Lind (1987) found a negative correlation between procedural justice and performance. Keller and Dansereau (1995) found a moderately strong relationship between procedural just Performance. Perhaps the most unclear of all relationships in the justice literature is the relationship between procedural justice and performance. For example, Earley and Lind (1987) found a relationship between procedural fairness judgments and perfor­mance in a laboratory study but not in a field study. Kanfer, Sawyer, Earley, and Lind (1987) found a negative correlation between procedural justice and performance. Keller and Dansereau (1995) found a moderately strong relationship between procedural just et al., 1994; Griffeth, Vecchio, & Logan, 1989). It is difficult to apply the logic of the agent-system and two-factor models to the prediction of performance. On the one hand, performance supports, and is often measured by, agents such as one’s supervisor. For this reason, Masterson, Lewis, et al. (2000) predicted, and found, stronger interactional justice effects on performance, consistent with the agent-system model. On the other hand, performance reflects members’ contributions to organizational goals (Borman, 1991), giving it a system-referenced character and suggesting that procedural justice should be its primary predictor. Different Operationalizations of Procedural Justice Different Operationalizations of Procedural Justice Of course, the relative strength of the correlations between the justice dimensions and the outcomes just described may depend on how procedural justice is operationalized. For example, some studies assess only process control but label it procedural justice (e.g., Joy & Witt, 1992). Others use a variable that is composed only of Leventhal criteria, labeling that procedural justice (e.g., Konovsky & Folger, 1991). Still others measure informational or interpersonal justice as the process variable (e.g., Gre Of course, the relative strength of the correlations between the justice dimensions and the outcomes just described may depend on how procedural justice is operationalized. For example, some studies assess only process control but label it procedural justice (e.g., Joy & Witt, 1992). Others use a variable that is composed only of Leventhal criteria, labeling that procedural justice (e.g., Konovsky & Folger, 1991). Still others measure informational or interpersonal justice as the process variable (e.g., Gre Bies, Martin, & Brockner, 1993; Colquitt & Chertkoff, 1996; Gilliland, 1994), which is what Lind and Tyler (1988) labeled a direct measure (i.e., a measure that directly asks respondents “how fair” a procedure is). Finally, another common operationalization is an indirect combination measure (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Such a measure asks respondents about some combination of process control, Leventhal criteria, and interpersonal or informational justice, labeling that procedural justice (e.g., Brockner et al., 1995, 1997; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). Presumably, those who use such measures subscribe to the view that interpersonal and informa­tional justice are facets of procedural justice, as with process control or Leventhal criteria. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 431 431 Our examination of the relative strength of the correlations between the different dimensions of justice and the outcomes described uses the broadest conceptualization of procedural justice by combining all of its potential operationalizations. However, our review also presents analyses broken down by operationalization. One strength of meta-analysis is that it allows a researcher to examine the degree to which a relationship varies across a partic­ular moderator variable. Our review examines operationaliza Our examination of the relative strength of the correlations between the different dimensions of justice and the outcomes described uses the broadest conceptualization of procedural justice by combining all of its potential operationalizations. However, our review also presents analyses broken down by operationalization. One strength of meta-analysis is that it allows a researcher to examine the degree to which a relationship varies across a partic­ular moderator variable. Our review examines operationaliza Method Method We conducted a meta-analytic review of the literature on organizational justice to examine the questions discussed earlier. In the following sec­tions, we review the methods for this review. We conducted a meta-analytic review of the literature on organizational justice to examine the questions discussed earlier. In the following sec­tions, we review the methods for this review. Literature Search Literature Search We first performed a literature search of the PsycINFO database for the years 1975 to 1999. Our starting date coincided with the year in which Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the procedural justice construct. Given that a major goal of this review was to examine the relative impact of multiple dimensions of justice, articles published before 1975 would be less useful. Such articles were typically proactive distributive justice arti­cles that examined how individuals allocate rewards across different si We first performed a literature search of the PsycINFO database for the years 1975 to 1999. Our starting date coincided with the year in which Thibaut and Walker (1975) introduced the procedural justice construct. Given that a major goal of this review was to examine the relative impact of multiple dimensions of justice, articles published before 1975 would be less useful. Such articles were typically proactive distributive justice arti­cles that examined how individuals allocate rewards across different si The search yielded 300 articles. Of these 300 articles, 76 did not include a relationship in the meta-analysis and were deemed not relevant. For example, Tyler, Lind, Ohbuchi, Sugawara, and Huo (1998) examined the effects of ‘‘relational judgments” (an aggregate measure of procedural, interpersonal, and informational justice judgments) on the willingness of disputants to accept a third-party solution in a conflict resolution setting. Bazerman, The search yielded 300 articles. Of these 300 articles, 76 did not include a relationship in the meta-analysis and were deemed not relevant. For example, Tyler, Lind, Ohbuchi, Sugawara, and Huo (1998) examined the effects of ‘‘relational judgments” (an aggregate measure of procedural, interpersonal, and informational justice judgments) on the willingness of disputants to accept a third-party solution in a conflict resolution setting. Bazerman, Schroth, Shah, Diekmann, and Tenbrunsel (1994) examined the effects of justice on job search decisions. Such studies did not focus on one of the nine outcome variables included in our review or did not include a relationship between different dimensions of justice. Another 41 studies included a relationship in the meta-analysis but could not be coded because of the way in which the results were presented. Meta-analysis requires zero-order effect size information, whether in the form of correlations, Another 41 studies included a relationship in the meta-analysis but could not be coded because of the way in which the results were presented. Meta-analysis requires zero-order effect size information, whether in the form of correlations, F statistics, t statistics, or even means and standard deviations of a dependent variable across multiple experimental conditions. Articles that present only partial or semipartial unique, independent effects cannot be coded unless the independent variables are uncorrelated, as in most experiments in which conditions are manipulated orthogonal to one another. Several articles uncovered in our literature review could not be coded because they presented only multiple regression or structural equa­tion modeling results. Examples included several of Tyler’s investigations of the “relational” or "group-value" model of justice (e.g., Tyler, 1989, 1994; Tyler, Degoey, & Smith, 1996). Other articles could not be coded because they presented incomplete results (e.g., only labeling coded because they presented only multiple regression or structural equa­tion modeling results. Examples included several of Tyler’s investigations of the “relational” or "group-value" model of justice (e.g., Tyler, 1989, 1994; Tyler, Degoey, & Smith, 1996). Other articles could not be coded because they presented incomplete results (e.g., only labeling F statistics in an analysis of variance as significant or not significant) or combined procedural and distributive justice into a larger justice variable (e.g., Martocchio & Judge, 1995). The final number of studies included in the review was 183. These studies are marked with an asterisk in the References section. We should note that this number included both laboratory studies and field studies; the term organizational justice in no way implies that the research must occur in a field setting but indicates only that the research must be relevant to the role of fairness in the workplace (Greenberg, 1990b). The final number of studies included in the review was 183. These studies are marked with an asterisk in the References section. We should note that this number included both laboratory studies and field studies; the term organizational justice in no way implies that the research must occur in a field setting but indicates only that the research must be relevant to the role of fairness in the workplace (Greenberg, 1990b). Meta-Analysis Strategy Meta-Analysis Strategy Meta-analysis is a technique that allows individual study results to be aggregated while correcting for various artifacts that can bias relationship estimates. Our meta-analyses were conducted with Hunter and Schmidt’s (1990) procedures. Inputs into the meta-analyses included zero-order effect sizes in the form of correlations, along with sample sizes and reliability information. In instances in which articles reported statistics other than correlations (e.g., Meta-analysis is a technique that allows individual study results to be aggregated while correcting for various artifacts that can bias relationship estimates. Our meta-analyses were conducted with Hunter and Schmidt’s (1990) procedures. Inputs into the meta-analyses included zero-order effect sizes in the form of correlations, along with sample sizes and reliability information. In instances in which articles reported statistics other than correlations (e.g., F statistics, t statistics, or means and standard devia­tions), results were transformed into correlations through the formulas provided by Hunter and Schmidt (1990). Because of the subjectivity and judgment calls inherent in meta-analytic efforts, all coding of meta-analytic data was performed by author dyads formed from the study’s five authors. In cases in which a variable was assessed with multiple measures, we acted in accordance with Hunter and Schmidt’s (1990) recommendations for conceptual replication (see pp. 451-463). Specifically, when multiple measures were highly correlated and seemed to each be construct valid, the correlations were averaged together (see Hunter & Schmidt, 1990, p. 457). Thus, one composite correlation was formed in lieu of multiple correla­tions, preventing a study that involved multiple measures from being “double cou In cases in which a variable was assessed with multiple measures, we acted in accordance with Hunter and Schmidt’s (1990) recommendations for conceptual replication (see pp. 451-463). Specifically, when multiple measures were highly correlated and seemed to each be construct valid, the correlations were averaged together (see Hunter & Schmidt, 1990, p. 457). Thus, one composite correlation was formed in lieu of multiple correla­tions, preventing a study that involved multiple measures from being “double cou Meta-analysis requires that each observed correlation from a given study be weighted by that study’s sample size to provide a weighted mean estimate of the population correlation. The standard deviation of this estimate across the multiple studies is also computed. This variation is composed of true variation in the population value as well as variation due to artifacts such as sampling error and measurement error. To provide a more accurate estimate of each population correlation and its variability, our a Meta-analysis requires that each observed correlation from a given study be weighted by that study’s sample size to provide a weighted mean estimate of the population correlation. The standard deviation of this estimate across the multiple studies is also computed. This variation is composed of true variation in the population value as well as variation due to artifacts such as sampling error and measurement error. To provide a more accurate estimate of each population correlation and its variability, our a Meta-analysis results typically include both uncorrected (r) and cor­rected (rc) estimates of the population correlation. The latter are corrected for unreliability in both variables. Also reported are the 95% confidence intervals for each population correlation. Confidence intervals are gener­ated with the standard error of the weighted mean correlation. They reflect the “extent to which sampling error remains in the estimate of a mean effect size” and are applied to estimates that have not been corrected Meta-analysis results typically include both uncorrected (r) and cor­rected (rc) estimates of the population correlation. The latter are corrected for unreliability in both variables. Also reported are the 95% confidence intervals for each population correlation. Confidence intervals are gener­ated with the standard error of the weighted mean correlation. They reflect the “extent to which sampling error remains in the estimate of a mean effect size” and are applied to estimates that have not been corrected Also presented is the standard deviation of the population correlation (SDrc). This provides an index of the variation in the corrected population Also presented is the standard deviation of the population correlation (SDrc). This provides an index of the variation in the corrected population 432 432 432 432 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG values across the studies in our sample. One indication that moderators may be present in a given relationship is the case in which artifacts such as unreliability fail to account for a substantial portion of the variance in correlations. Thus, the percentage of variance explained by artifacts (VOT) is also presented. Hunter and Schmidt (1990) have suggested that, if artifacts fail to account for 75% of the variance in the correlations, moderators probably exist. Mathieu and Zajac (1990) and Hom, values across the studies in our sample. One indication that moderators may be present in a given relationship is the case in which artifacts such as unreliability fail to account for a substantial portion of the variance in correlations. Thus, the percentage of variance explained by artifacts (VOT) is also presented. Hunter and Schmidt (1990) have suggested that, if artifacts fail to account for 75% of the variance in the correlations, moderators probably exist. Mathieu and Zajac (1990) and Hom, Caranikas­ Walker, Prussia, and Griffeth (1992) amended the 75% criterion to 60% in cases in which range restriction is not one of the artifacts that is corrected for. We should note that the 60% rule only implies the existence of a moderator; it does not indicate what variable is acting as the moderator. Identifying the actual moderator variable requires coding potential mod­erators and then performing subgroup analyses to determine whether the population correlations vary across the subgroup boundaries. In our review, this Meta-Analytic Regression Strategy Meta-Analytic Regression Strategy Many questions cannot be answered by a matrix of meta-analyzed correlations. For example, do Leventhal’s (1980) justice criteria explain variance in procedural fairness perceptions beyond the effects of Thibaut and Walker’s (1975) process control construct? Do procedural, distribu­tive, interpersonal, and informational justice each have independent effects on organizational commitment? Such questions require multiple regression to assess independent, semipartial effects. Fortunately, recent advances in meta Many questions cannot be answered by a matrix of meta-analyzed correlations. For example, do Leventhal’s (1980) justice criteria explain variance in procedural fairness perceptions beyond the effects of Thibaut and Walker’s (1975) process control construct? Do procedural, distribu­tive, interpersonal, and informational justice each have independent effects on organizational commitment? Such questions require multiple regression to assess independent, semipartial effects. Fortunately, recent advances in meta et al., 1992), and job performance (Schmidt, Hunter, & Outerbridge, 1986). In practice, there are some decision points that researchers using meta- analytic regression will encounter (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). First, many researchers will conduct regressions on correlation matrices that vary in sample size. This raises the question of what sample size to use when computing the standard errors associated with the regression weights. Potential solutions include using the smallest cell sample size or using the mean sample size. We chose to use the harmonic mean of the correlation ma In practice, there are some decision points that researchers using meta- analytic regression will encounter (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). First, many researchers will conduct regressions on correlation matrices that vary in sample size. This raises the question of what sample size to use when computing the standard errors associated with the regression weights. Potential solutions include using the smallest cell sample size or using the mean sample size. We chose to use the harmonic mean of the correlation ma к refers to the number of study correlations and N refers to the sample sizes of the studies. An inspection of the formula shows that the harmonic mean gives much less weight to substantially large individual study sample sizes, so it is always more conservative than the arithmetic mean (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). Note, however, that the sample size issue is not very critical to the current meta-analytic review. This is because the sample sizes for almost all of the cells of the correlation matrices are in th Second, researchers must choose whether to use maximum-likelihood estimation (the choice of Hom Second, researchers must choose whether to use maximum-likelihood estimation (the choice of Hom et al., 1992), ordinary least squares (the choice of Colquitt et al., 2000, Podsakoff et al., 1996, and Schmidt et al., 1986), or some other method. We elected to use ordinary least squares estimation, consistent with Colquitt et al. (2000), Podsakoff et al. (1996), and Schmidt et al. (1986). Ordinary least squares assumptions are less restrictive than maximum likelihood, which assumes multivariate normal­ity. Maximum-likelihood estimation is also less optimal in instances in which the data are in the form of correlations rather than covariances (Cudeck, 1989). Given that meta-analysis results in correlational data, we believed that ordinary least squares would be more appropriate. Results Results Construct Discrimination Questions Construct Discrimination Questions Research Question I concerned the magnitude of the relation­ships among the various organizational justice conceptualizations. Table 1 shows the correlations among process control, Leventhal criteria, interpersonal justice, informational justice, distributive justice, and procedural fairness perceptions. Cohen and Cohen (1983) classified correlations as high, moderate, or weak according to uncorrected r values of .50, .30, and .10, respectively. Thus, Table 1 shows high correlations among the justice concep Research Question I concerned the magnitude of the relation­ships among the various organizational justice conceptualizations. Table 1 shows the correlations among process control, Leventhal criteria, interpersonal justice, informational justice, distributive justice, and procedural fairness perceptions. Cohen and Cohen (1983) classified correlations as high, moderate, or weak according to uncorrected r values of .50, .30, and .10, respectively. Thus, Table 1 shows high correlations among the justice concep Table 1 also shows the correlations among the interactional justice dimensions. Interpersonal justice was highly related to informational justice (r = .57, rc = .66), although again not so highly that the two necessarily seem to be indicators of the same underlying construct. Interpersonal justice and informational jus­tice were also highly correlated with procedural fairness percep­tions (r = .56, rc = .63 and r = .51, rc = .58, respectively). These relationships were similar in magnitude to the process co Table 1 also shows the correlations among the interactional justice dimensions. Interpersonal justice was highly related to informational justice (r = .57, rc = .66), although again not so highly that the two necessarily seem to be indicators of the same underlying construct. Interpersonal justice and informational jus­tice were also highly correlated with procedural fairness percep­tions (r = .56, rc = .63 and r = .51, rc = .58, respectively). These relationships were similar in magnitude to the process co Interestingly, the relationships between interpersonal and infor­mational justice and procedural fairness perceptions were not significantly stronger than the relationship between distributive justice and procedural fairness perceptions (r = .48, rc = .57). This suggests that interpersonal and informational justice should be considered to be distinct from procedural justice, just as the case with distributive justice. The uncorrected correlation of .48 for distributive justice and procedural fairness percep Interestingly, the relationships between interpersonal and infor­mational justice and procedural fairness perceptions were not significantly stronger than the relationship between distributive justice and procedural fairness perceptions (r = .48, rc = .57). This suggests that interpersonal and informational justice should be considered to be distinct from procedural justice, just as the case with distributive justice. The uncorrected correlation of .48 for distributive justice and procedural fairness percep Combining all operationalizations, broadly defined procedural justice was strongly related to distributive justice (r = .56, rc = .67). The high standard deviation of the population correlation Combining all operationalizations, broadly defined procedural justice was strongly related to distributive justice (r = .56, rc = .67). The high standard deviation of the population correlation META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 433 433 Figure Table 1 Table 1 Table 1 Relationships Among Different Conceptualizations of Organizational Justice Relationships Among Different Conceptualizations of Organizational Justice ä ä «ï «ï c о ω ω Q G 05 W W 73 C C сЗ «'S «'S G О C C О О о о t с «л «л d О -2 ë О -2 ë О оз 3 оз 3 а о а о & о & о ä э а «3 э а «3 с о с о я я — 73 — 73 Я 5 Я 5 S* S* о _ 2" л 2" л (SDrc (SDrc = .23) and the low variance explained from artifacts (Vatt = 2.74%) show that moderators exist in the broadly defined procedural justice-distributive justice relationship. Operational­ization explains some of that variation, in that the highest relation­ship was evident when an indirect combination measure was used 0 = .63, r. = .77). Moreover, because their confidence intervals do not overlap, Table 2 shows that the indirect combination mea­sure relationship is significantly stronger than the more unidimen­sional justice measures: procedural fairness perceptions (r = .48, rc = .57), process control (r = .27, rc = .34), Leventhal criteria (r = .46, r, = .55), interpersonal justice (r = .38, rc = .42), and informational justice (r = .39, rc = .46). Proactive Research Questions Proactive Research Questions Research Question 2 explored whether the additional conceptu­alizations of justice over the course of its history have contributed incremental variance explained in regard to promoting procedural fairness perceptions. This was tested by regressing procedural fairness perceptions onto the various justice operationalizations using hierarchical regression. Order of entry was based on histor­ical introduction, so the following steps were used: (a) Thibaut and Walker’s (1975) process control operationalization, Research Question 2 explored whether the additional conceptu­alizations of justice over the course of its history have contributed incremental variance explained in regard to promoting procedural fairness perceptions. This was tested by regressing procedural fairness perceptions onto the various justice operationalizations using hierarchical regression. Order of entry was based on histor­ical introduction, so the following steps were used: (a) Thibaut and Walker’s (1975) process control operationalization, Bies and Moag’s (1986) interactional justice construct, broken down into interpersonal and informational justice (as in Greenberg, 1993b). The meta-analytic regression results are shown in Table 3. The original operationalization of procedural justice, process control, explained 26% of the variance in procedural fairness perceptions, with higher levels of process control producing more favorable fairness perceptions The meta-analytic regression results are shown in Table 3. The original operationalization of procedural justice, process control, explained 26% of the variance in procedural fairness perceptions, with higher levels of process control producing more favorable fairness perceptions (ß = .51). Leventhal criteria explained an additional 21% of the variance, with higher levels of his criteria also producing more favorable perceptions (ß = .61). Interper­sonal and informational justice explained an additional 6% of the variance in fairness perceptions. That effect was due primarily to interpersonal justice (ß = .29), although informational justice also had a significant effect (ß = .11). Research Question 3 asked how much variance the justice operationalizations explained in procedural fairness perceptions beyond the effects of distributive justice. This was tested by regressing procedural fairness perceptions onto the various justice operationalizations after controlling for distributive justice. These regression results are shown in Table 4. Distributive justice ex­plained 33% of the variance in procedural fairness perceptions, with higher levels of distributive justice being associated w Research Question 3 asked how much variance the justice operationalizations explained in procedural fairness perceptions beyond the effects of distributive justice. This was tested by regressing procedural fairness perceptions onto the various justice operationalizations after controlling for distributive justice. These regression results are shown in Table 4. Distributive justice ex­plained 33% of the variance in procedural fairness perceptions, with higher levels of distributive justice being associated w (ß = .57). The remaining justice operationalizations explained an incremental 24% of the variance. However, only two operationalizations had strong unique effects: Leventhal criteria (ß = .30) and interpersonal justice (ß = .26). Although statistically significant, the unique effects of pro­cess control (ß = .03) and informational justice (ß = .07) were not practically significant. Reactive Research Questions Reactive Research Questions Research Questions 4-6 explored the relationships between the organizational justice dimensions and several key outcome vari­ables. Table 5 presents the results for these research questions. Research Question 4 explored the relationships between Research Questions 4-6 explored the relationships between the organizational justice dimensions and several key outcome vari­ables. Table 5 presents the results for these research questions. Research Question 4 explored the relationships between distribu- 434 434 434 434 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG Table 2 Table 2 Table 2 Table 2 Relationship Between Distributive and Procedural Justice Conceptualizations (Including Interpersonal and Informational Justice) Relationship Between Distributive and Procedural Justice Conceptualizations (Including Interpersonal and Informational Justice) Conceptualization Conceptualization Conceptualization Conceptualization Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice TR r (95% CI) r (95% CI) r (95% CI) N (SDrO N (SDrO N (SDrO k k k (N) Vm (%) Vm (%) Vm (%) Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice .56 (.52, .60) .56 (.52, .60) .56 (.52, .60) .67 (.23) .67 (.23) .67 (.23) 92 (42,576) 92 (42,576) 92 (42,576) 2.74 2.74 2.74 Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions .48 (.41, .55) .48 (.41, .55) .48 (.41, .55) .57 (.28) .57 (.28) .57 (.28) 45(13,418) 45(13,418) 45(13,418) 3.41 3.41 3.41 Process control Process control Process control Process control .27 (.22, .33) .27 (.22, .33) .27 (.22, .33) .34 (.13) .34 (.13) .34 (.13) 23(5,137) 23(5,137) 23(5,137) 24.15 24.15 24.15 Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria .46 (.38, .54) .46 (.38, .54) .46 (.38, .54) .55 (.17) .55 (.17) .55 (.17) 15 (4,743) 15 (4,743) 15 (4,743) 8.27 8.27 8.27 Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice .38 (.26, .50) .38 (.26, .50) .38 (.26, .50) .42 (.20) .42 (.20) .42 (.20) 9 (3,496) 9 (3,496) 9 (3,496) 5.62 5.62 5.62 informational justice informational justice informational justice informational justice .39 (.32, .47) .39 (.32, .47) .39 (.32, .47) .46 (.15) .46 (.15) .46 (.15) 14 (3,807) 14 (3,807) 14 (3,807) 13.19 13.19 13.19 Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure .63 (.59, .66) .63 (.59, .66) .63 (.59, .66) .77 (.14) .77 (.14) .77 (.14) 54 (51,446) 54 (51,446) 54 (51,446) 2.32 2.32 2.32 Note, r = uncorrected population correlation; Cl = confidence interval around uncorrected population correlation; rc = corrected population correlation; SDrc = standard deviation of corrected population correlation; Note, r = uncorrected population correlation; Cl = confidence interval around uncorrected population correlation; rc = corrected population correlation; SDrc = standard deviation of corrected population correlation; к = number of studies; Vart = percentage of variance in rc explained by study artifacts. tive justice and the outcomes. Distributive justice had high corre­lations with outcome satisfaction (r = .52, rc — .61), job satisfac­tion (r = .46, rc = .56), organizational commitment (r = .42, r,. = .51), trust (r = .48, rc = .57), agent-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .53, = .59), and withdrawal (r = —.41, rc = tive justice and the outcomes. Distributive justice had high corre­lations with outcome satisfaction (r = .52, rc — .61), job satisfac­tion (r = .46, rc = .56), organizational commitment (r = .42, r,. = .51), trust (r = .48, rc = .57), agent-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .53, = .59), and withdrawal (r = —.41, rc = —.50). Distributive justice had moderate correlations with system- referenced evaluation of authority (r = .30, rc = .37), OCBOs (r = .20, rc = .25), and negative reactions (r = —.26, ra = —.30) and was weakly related to OCBIs (r = .13, rc = .15) and performance (r = .13, rc = .15). —.50). Distributive justice had moderate correlations with system- referenced evaluation of authority (r = .30, rc = .37), OCBOs (r = .20, rc = .25), and negative reactions (r = —.26, ra = —.30) and was weakly related to OCBIs (r = .13, rc = .15) and performance (r = .13, rc = .15). Research Question 5 explored relationships between procedural justice and the same set of outcomes. Combining all conceivable procedural justice conceptualizations, again labeled broadly de­fined procedural justice, we see that procedural justice had high correlations with outcome satisfaction (r = .40, rc = .48), job satisfaction (r = .51, rc = .62), organizational commitment (r = .48, rc = .57), trust (r = .52, rc = .61), and agent-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .56, rc = .64). Procedural justice Research Question 5 explored relationships between procedural justice and the same set of outcomes. Combining all conceivable procedural justice conceptualizations, again labeled broadly de­fined procedural justice, we see that procedural justice had high correlations with outcome satisfaction (r = .40, rc = .48), job satisfaction (r = .51, rc = .62), organizational commitment (r = .48, rc = .57), trust (r = .52, rc = .61), and agent-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .56, rc = .64). Procedural justice had weak correlations with OCBIs (r = .19, rc = .22). Table 5 also shows that moderators were present in the procedural justice­outcome relationships, with the exception of the relationships with the OCB variables. Operationalization could be one potential mod­erator for several of the outcome variables, because the indirect had weak correlations with OCBIs (r = .19, rc = .22). Table 5 also shows that moderators were present in the procedural justice­outcome relationships, with the exception of the relationships with the OCB variables. Operationalization could be one potential mod­erator for several of the outcome variables, because the indirect Table 3 Table 3 Table 3 Table 3 Incremental Effects of Justice Operationalizations on Incremental Effects of Justice Operationalizations on Procedural Fairness Perceptions Procedural Fairness Perceptions Regression step Regression step Regression step Regression step Total R2 Total R2 Total R2 Δ/?2 Δ/?2 Δ/?2 В В В 1. Process control 1. Process control 1. Process control 1. Process control .26* .26* .26* .26* .26* .26* .51* .51* .51* 2. Leventhal criteria 2. Leventhal criteria 2. Leventhal criteria 2. Leventhal criteria .47* .47* .47* .21* .21* .21* .61* .61* .61* 3. Interpersonal justice 3. Interpersonal justice 3. Interpersonal justice 3. Interpersonal justice .53* .53* .53* .06* .06* .06* .28* .28* .28* Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice .11* .11* .11* Note. N - 4,165 (based on the harmonic mean sample size of the Table 1 cells used in this regression). Note. N - 4,165 (based on the harmonic mean sample size of the Table 1 cells used in this regression). *p< .05. *p< .05. combination measure was significantly more related to job satis­faction, organizational commitment, and withdrawal than was any other operationalization. combination measure was significantly more related to job satis­faction, organizational commitment, and withdrawal than was any other operationalization. Research Question 6 explored relationships between interper­sonal and informational justice and the outcome variables. Inter­personal justice was strongly related to agent-referenced evalua­tion of authority (r = .57, rc = .62) and moderately related to job satisfaction (r = .31, rc = .35), system-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .20, rc = .23), OCBIs (r = .23, rc = .29), and negative reactions (r = —.30, rc = —.35). It was weakly related to outcome satisfaction (r = .19), organizational commitment ( Research Question 6 explored relationships between interper­sonal and informational justice and the outcome variables. Inter­personal justice was strongly related to agent-referenced evalua­tion of authority (r = .57, rc = .62) and moderately related to job satisfaction (r = .31, rc = .35), system-referenced evaluation of authority (r = .20, rc = .23), OCBIs (r = .23, rc = .29), and negative reactions (r = —.30, rc = —.35). It was weakly related to outcome satisfaction (r = .19), organizational commitment ( Research Question 7 examined the unique effects of the justice dimensions on the outcome variables considered simultaneously. Table 6 presents beta weights derived from regressing each of the outcomes on all four justice dimensions simultaneously. The table also presents unique R2 values derived from a “usefulness analy­sis” in which the effects of one justice dimension were examined after controlling for the others. The table also presents the total variance explained in each outcome by the justice dimensi Research Question 7 examined the unique effects of the justice dimensions on the outcome variables considered simultaneously. Table 6 presents beta weights derived from regressing each of the outcomes on all four justice dimensions simultaneously. The table also presents unique R2 values derived from a “usefulness analy­sis” in which the effects of one justice dimension were examined after controlling for the others. The table also presents the total variance explained in each outcome by the justice dimensi The regression results in Table 6 allowed us to examine the adequacy of the distributive dominance, two-factor, and agent­system reactive models. Leventhal’s (1980) distributive domi­nance model would predict higher unique effects for The regression results in Table 6 allowed us to examine the adequacy of the distributive dominance, two-factor, and agent­system reactive models. Leventhal’s (1980) distributive domi­nance model would predict higher unique effects for distribu- META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 435 435 Table 4 Table 4 Table 4 Table 4 Incremental Effects of Justice Operationalizations on Procedural Incremental Effects of Justice Operationalizations on Procedural Fairness Perceptions Beyond Distributive Justice Effects Fairness Perceptions Beyond Distributive Justice Effects Regression step Regression step Regression step Regression step Total R2 Total R2 Total R2 Δ«2 Δ«2 Δ«2 В В В 1. Distributive justice 1. Distributive justice 1. Distributive justice 1. Distributive justice .33* .33* .33* .33* .33* .33* .57* .57* .57* 2. Process control 2. Process control 2. Process control 2. Process control .57* .57* .57* .24* .24* .24* .03* .03* .03* Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria .30* .30* .30* Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice .26* .26* .26* Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice .07* .07* .07* Note. N = 4,165 (based on the harmonic mean sample size of the Table 1 cells used in this regression). Note. N = 4,165 (based on the harmonic mean sample size of the Table 1 cells used in this regression). * p < .05. * p < .05. tive justice than for the other three justice forms. This prediction was supported for two outcomes (outcome satisfac­tion and withdrawal) and was refuted for the remaining nine outcomes. tive justice than for the other three justice forms. This prediction was supported for two outcomes (outcome satisfac­tion and withdrawal) and was refuted for the remaining nine outcomes. Sweeney and McFarlin’s (1993) two-factor model would predict higher unique effects of distributive justice for person-referenced outcomes and higher unique effects of procedural justice for system-referenced outcomes. This prediction was supported for five outcomes (outcome satisfaction, job satisfaction, organiza­tional commitment, system-referenced evaluation of authority, and performance) and was refuted for three outcomes (OCBOs, with­drawal, and negative reactions). The two-factor model was not relevan Sweeney and McFarlin’s (1993) two-factor model would predict higher unique effects of distributive justice for person-referenced outcomes and higher unique effects of procedural justice for system-referenced outcomes. This prediction was supported for five outcomes (outcome satisfaction, job satisfaction, organiza­tional commitment, system-referenced evaluation of authority, and performance) and was refuted for three outcomes (OCBOs, with­drawal, and negative reactions). The two-factor model was not relevan Bies Bies and Moag’s (1986) agent-system model would predict higher unique effects of interpersonal or informational justice for agent-referenced outcomes and higher unique effects of procedural justice for system-referenced outcomes. This prediction was sup­ported for five outcomes (job satisfaction, organizational commit­ment, agent-referenced evaluation of authority, OCBIs, and per­formance) and refuted for five outcomes (system-referenced evaluation of authority, trust, OCBOs, withdrawal, and negative reactions). Discussion Discussion This meta-analytic review, consisting of 120 separate meta­analyses of 183 empirical studies, explored three types of research questions to aid further theory development in the organizational justice literature. The first type of question was related to construct discrimination (i.e., how highly related are the various facets of organizational justice?). Process control and Leventhal criteria, the two original procedural justice operationalizations, were highly correlated, although perhaps not as highly as This meta-analytic review, consisting of 120 separate meta­analyses of 183 empirical studies, explored three types of research questions to aid further theory development in the organizational justice literature. The first type of question was related to construct discrimination (i.e., how highly related are the various facets of organizational justice?). Process control and Leventhal criteria, the two original procedural justice operationalizations, were highly correlated, although perhaps not as highly as relationship is strongest when an indirect combination measure is used and weaker when procedural fairness perceptions, Leventhal criteria, and especially process control are used. relationship is strongest when an indirect combination measure is used and weaker when procedural fairness perceptions, Leventhal criteria, and especially process control are used. The second type of research question dealt with proactive re­search, which is concerned with creating procedural fairness per­ceptions by adhering to certain rules, such as providing process control, being consistent, treating people with sensitivity, or ex­plaining things adequately. Our results showed that the historical progression of proactive research on procedural justice has in fact contributed to our ability to promote procedural fairness percep­tions. The original conceptualization, Thibaut and Wal The second type of research question dealt with proactive re­search, which is concerned with creating procedural fairness per­ceptions by adhering to certain rules, such as providing process control, being consistent, treating people with sensitivity, or ex­plaining things adequately. Our results showed that the historical progression of proactive research on procedural justice has in fact contributed to our ability to promote procedural fairness percep­tions. The original conceptualization, Thibaut and Wal The concepts of interpersonal and informational justice, drawn from the field’s recent interest in interactional justice, show a more mixed pattern of results. Their correlations with procedural fair­ness perceptions were just as strong as the correlations for Lev­enthal criteria. However, although they explained significant in­cremental variance in fairness perceptions, their contribution was small in comparison with the contributions of the other justice facets. Thus, interpersonal and informational justi The concepts of interpersonal and informational justice, drawn from the field’s recent interest in interactional justice, show a more mixed pattern of results. Their correlations with procedural fair­ness perceptions were just as strong as the correlations for Lev­enthal criteria. However, although they explained significant in­cremental variance in fairness perceptions, their contribution was small in comparison with the contributions of the other justice facets. Thus, interpersonal and informational justi Because it is well known that the fairness of decision-making procedures is often judged according to the kinds of outcomes one receives (Lind & Tyler, 1988), it is also important to show that the various conceptualizations of procedural justice are important even after control for distributive justice. If this had not been the case, then the procedural justice literature would have contributed little over earlier work by Homans (1961), Adams (1965), Leventhal (1976), and Deutsch (1975). Our results showed Because it is well known that the fairness of decision-making procedures is often judged according to the kinds of outcomes one receives (Lind & Tyler, 1988), it is also important to show that the various conceptualizations of procedural justice are important even after control for distributive justice. If this had not been the case, then the procedural justice literature would have contributed little over earlier work by Homans (1961), Adams (1965), Leventhal (1976), and Deutsch (1975). Our results showed Turning to our third type of research question, reactive research, we tested three separate reactive models: Leventhal’s (1980) dis­tributive dominance model, Sweeney and McFarlin’s (1993) two- factor model, and Turning to our third type of research question, reactive research, we tested three separate reactive models: Leventhal’s (1980) dis­tributive dominance model, Sweeney and McFarlin’s (1993) two- factor model, and Bies and Moag’s (1986) agent-system model. Support for these models can be evaluated by examining the relative effects of distributive, procedural, interpersonal, and in­formational justice on the basis of the size of their meta-analytic correlations, as well as their unique effects in the meta-analytic regressions. We find little support for the distributive dominance 436 436 436 436 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG Table 5 Table 5 Table 5 Table 5 Relationships Between Organizational Justice Dimensions and Outcome Variables Relationships Between Organizational Justice Dimensions and Outcome Variables Dimension Dimension Dimension Dimension r (95% CI) r (95% CI) r (95% CI) % «DrJ % «DrJ % «DrJ k k k (N) van van van (%) (%) r (95% CI) r (95% CI) r (95% CI) % % % W к к к IN) vart (%) vart (%) vart (%) TR Outcome satisfaction Outcome satisfaction Outcome satisfaction Job satisfaction Job satisfaction Job satisfaction Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice .40 (.35, .46) .40 (.35, .46) .40 (.35, .46) .48 (.18) .48 (.18) .48 (.18) 30 (8,073) 30 (8,073) 30 (8,073) 10.02 10.02 10.02 .51 (.46, .56) .51 (.46, .56) .51 (.46, .56) .62 (.18) .62 (.18) .62 (.18) 40 (31,774) 40 (31,774) 40 (31,774) 2.88 2.88 2.88 Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions .45 (.36, .54) .45 (.36, .54) .45 (.36, .54) .53 (.17) .53 (.17) .53 (.17) 11 (4,420) 11 (4,420) 11 (4,420) 6.59 6.59 6.59 .33 (.28, .38) .33 (.28, .38) .33 (.28, .38) .40 (.09) .40 (.09) .40 (.09) 11 (4,958) 11 (4,958) 11 (4,958) 24.00 24.00 24.00 Process control Process control Process control Process control .38 (.27, .48) .38 (.27, .48) .38 (.27, .48) .45 (.16) .45 (.16) .45 (.16) 8(1,774) 8(1,774) 8(1,774) 14.74 14.74 14.74 .30 (.25, .35) .30 (.25, .35) .30 (.25, .35) .37 (.05) .37 (.05) .37 (.05) 5 (2,577) 5 (2,577) 5 (2,577) 50.39 50.39 50.39 Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria .25 (.13, .37) .25 (.13, .37) .25 (.13, .37) .32 (.17) .32 (.17) .32 (.17) 6(1,796) 6(1,796) 6(1,796) 13.03 13.03 13.03 .36 (.33, .40) .36 (.33, .40) .36 (.33, .40) .42 (.00) .42 (.00) .42 (.00) 4(2,315) 4(2,315) 4(2,315) 100.00 100.00 100.00 Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice .19 .19 .19 1 (301) 1 (301) 1 (301) .31 (.26,.36) .31 (.26,.36) .31 (.26,.36) .35 (.02) .35 (.02) .35 (.02) 2(1,795) 2(1,795) 2(1,795) 65.96 65.96 65.96 Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice .27 (.21, .33) .27 (.21, .33) .27 (.21, .33) .30 (.04) .30 (.04) .30 (.04) 4(1,404) 4(1,404) 4(1,404) 65.02 65.02 65.02 .38 (.34, .42) .38 (.34, .42) .38 (.34, .42) .43 (.00) .43 (.00) .43 (.00) 2(1,872) 2(1,872) 2(1,872) 100.00 100.00 100.00 Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure .47 (.39, .54) .47 (.39, .54) .47 (.39, .54) .54 (.15) .54 (.15) .54 (.15) 14 (2,242) 14 (2,242) 14 (2,242) 18.87 18.87 18.87 .55 (.49, .61) .55 (.49, .61) .55 (.49, .61) .68 (.17) .68 (.17) .68 (.17) 22(25,221) 22(25,221) 22(25,221) 2.07 2.07 2.07 Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice .52 (.46, .59) .52 (.46, .59) .52 (.46, .59) .61 (.20) .61 (.20) .61 (.20) 28 (9,321) 28 (9,321) 28 (9,321) 5.07 5.07 5.07 .46 (.42, .49) .46 (.42, .49) .46 (.42, .49) .56 (.09) .56 (.09) .56 (.09) 24 (57,515) 24 (57,515) 24 (57,515) 8.31 8.31 8.31 TR Organizational commitment Organizational commitment Organizational commitment Trust Trust Trust Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice .48 (.44, .52) .48 (.44, .52) .48 (.44, .52) .57 (.18) .57 (.18) .57 (.18) 53 (33,455) 53 (33,455) 53 (33,455) 3.84 3.84 3.84 .52 (.44, .59) .52 (.44, .59) .52 (.44, .59) .61 (.20) .61 (.20) .61 (.20) 24 (4,522) 24 (4,522) 24 (4,522) 8.42 8.42 8.42 Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions .32 (.25, .39) .32 (.25, .39) .32 (.25, .39) .37 (.16) .37 (.16) .37 (.16) 18 (6,767) 18 (6,767) 18 (6,767) 9.58 9.58 9.58 .56 (.49, .64) .56 (.49, .64) .56 (.49, .64) .62 (.08) .62 (.08) .62 (.08) 7 (802) 7 (802) 7 (802) 39.78 39.78 39.78 Process control Process control Process control Process control .22 (.18, .27) .22 (.18, .27) .22 (.18, .27) .27 (.05) .27 (.05) .27 (.05) 7 (2,898) 7 (2,898) 7 (2,898) 53.50 53.50 53.50 .40 (.30, .50) .40 (.30, .50) .40 (.30, .50) .47 (.13) .47 (.13) .47 (.13) 7(1,031) 7(1,031) 7(1,031) 26.54 26.54 26.54 Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria .26 (.22, .29) .26 (.22, .29) .26 (.22, .29) .30 (.00) .30 (.00) .30 (.00) 5 (3,162) 5 (3,162) 5 (3,162) 100.00 100.00 100.00 .58 (.09, .99) .58 (.09, .99) .58 (.09, .99) .65 (.38) .65 (.38) .65 (.38) 2 (628) 2 (628) 2 (628) 1.23 1.23 1.23 Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice .16 (.11, .20) .16 (.11, .20) .16 (.11, .20) .19 (.00) .19 (.00) .19 (.00) 2(1,824) 2(1,824) 2(1,824) 100.00 100.00 100.00 Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice .26 (.18, .34) .26 (.18, .34) .26 (.18, .34) .29 (.14) .29 (.14) .29 (.14) 10 (3,968) 10 (3,968) 10 (3,968) 12.01 12.01 12.01 .43 (.30, .57) .43 (.30, .57) .43 (.30, .57) .51 (.11) .51 (.11) .51 (.11) 3 (487) 3 (487) 3 (487) 30.21 30.21 30.21 Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure .55 (.51, .58) .55 (.51, .58) .55 (.51, .58) .65 (.10) .65 (.10) .65 (.10) 26 (24,606) 26 (24,606) 26 (24,606) 6.10 6.10 6.10 .55 (.45, .66) .55 (.45, .66) .55 (.45, .66) .64 (.18) .64 (.18) .64 (.18) 10(2,169) 10(2,169) 10(2,169) 8.02 8.02 8.02 Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice .42 (.38, .47) .42 (.38, .47) .42 (.38, .47) .51 (.13) .51 (.13) .51 (.13) 24 (27,805) 24 (27,805) 24 (27,805) 4.11 4.11 4.11 .48 (.40, .57) .48 (.40, .57) .48 (.40, .57) .57 (.13) .57 (.13) .57 (.13) 8 (1,735) 8 (1,735) 8 (1,735) 17.24 17.24 17.24 TR Evaluation of authority: Agent referenced Evaluation of authority: Agent referenced Evaluation of authority: Agent referenced Evaluation of authority: System referenced Evaluation of authority: System referenced Evaluation of authority: System referenced Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice .56 (.52, .59) .56 (.52, .59) .56 (.52, .59) .64 (.11) .64 (.11) .64 (.11) 33 (20,034) 33 (20,034) 33 (20,034) 7.17 7.17 7.17 .35 (-31,-40) .35 (-31,-40) .35 (-31,-40) .42 (.13) .42 (.13) .42 (.13) 25 (9,708) 25 (9,708) 25 (9,708) 13.09 13.09 13.09 Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions .53 (.46, .60) .53 (.46, .60) .53 (.46, .60) .60 (.13) .60 (.13) .60 (.13) 13 (4,753) 13 (4,753) 13 (4,753) 8.76 8.76 8.76 .45 (.42, .48) .45 (.42, .48) .45 (.42, .48) .51 (.12) .51 (.12) .51 (.12) 14 (5,637) 14 (5,637) 14 (5,637) 11.63 11.63 11.63 Process control Process control Process control Process control .42 (.35, .50) .42 (.35, .50) .42 (.35, .50) .50 (.14) .50 (.14) .50 (.14) 10 (3,436) 10 (3,436) 10 (3,436) 12.56 12.56 12.56 .12 (.05, .20) .12 (.05, .20) .12 (.05, .20) .16 (.00) .16 (.00) .16 (.00) 3(663) 3(663) 3(663) 100.00 100.00 100.00 Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria .53 (.41, .64) .53 (.41, .64) .53 (.41, .64) .63 (.17) .63 (.17) .63 (.17) 7 (3,104) 7 (3,104) 7 (3,104) 4.86 4.86 4.86 .19 (.02, .36) .19 (.02, .36) .19 (.02, .36) .22 (.15) .22 (.15) .22 (.15) 3 (427) 3 (427) 3 (427) 29.27 29.27 29.27 Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice .57 (.43, .71) .57 (.43, .71) .57 (.43, .71) .62 (.17) .62 (.17) .62 (.17) 5 (2,534) 5 (2,534) 5 (2,534) 3.44 3.44 3.44 .20 (.01, .38) .20 (.01, .38) .20 (.01, .38) .23 (.16) .23 (.16) .23 (.16) 3 (469) 3 (469) 3 (469) 22.95 22.95 22.95 Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice .58 (.46, .71) .58 (.46, .71) .58 (.46, .71) .65 (.21) .65 (.21) .65 (.21) 9 (3,210) 9 (3,210) 9 (3,210) 3.21 3.21 3.21 .42 (.29, .55) .42 (.29, .55) .42 (.29, .55) .47 (.15) .47 (.15) .47 (.15) 5(1,035) 5(1,035) 5(1,035) 14.94 14.94 14.94 Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure .58 (.54, .62) .58 (.54, .62) .58 (.54, .62) .67 (.05) .67 (.05) .67 (.05) 9 (13,850) 9 (13,850) 9 (13,850) 6.65 6.65 6.65 .31 (.24, .38) .31 (.24, .38) .31 (.24, .38) .37 (.12) .37 (.12) .37 (.12) 11 (3,790) 11 (3,790) 11 (3,790) 17.96 17.96 17.96 Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice .53 (.46, .61) .53 (.46, .61) .53 (.46, .61) .59 (.15) .59 (.15) .59 (.15) 13 (16,963) 13 (16,963) 13 (16,963) 1.96 1.96 1.96 .30 (.24, .36) .30 (.24, .36) .30 (.24, .36) .37 (.12) .37 (.12) .37 (.12) 13 (4,103) 13 (4,103) 13 (4,103) 20.64 20.64 20.64 TR OCBs: Individual referenced OCBs: Individual referenced OCBs: Individual referenced OCBs: Organization referenced OCBs: Organization referenced OCBs: Organization referenced Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice .19 (.16, .22) .19 (.16, .22) .19 (.16, .22) .22 (.00) .22 (.00) .22 (.00) 15 (4,414) 15 (4,414) 15 (4,414) 100.00 100.00 100.00 .23 (.19, .26) .23 (.19, .26) .23 (.19, .26) .27 (.04) .27 (.04) .27 (.04) 15(3,176) 15(3,176) 15(3,176) 78.62 78.62 78.62 Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions .21 (.17, .25) .21 (.17, .25) .21 (.17, .25) .25 (.00) .25 (.00) .25 (.00) 2(1,872) 2(1,872) 2(1,872) 100.00 100.00 100.00 .21 .21 .21 1 (140) 1 (140) 1 (140) Process control Process control Process control Process control .16 (.12, .20) .16 (.12, .20) .16 (.12, .20) .21 (.00) .21 (.00) .21 (.00) 3 (2,000) 3 (2,000) 3 (2,000) 100.00 100.00 100.00 .14 .14 .14 1 (206) 1 (206) 1 (206) Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria .18 (.14, .23) .18 (.14, .23) .18 (.14, .23) .22 (.01) .22 (.01) .22 (.01) 3 (2,108) 3 (2,108) 3 (2,108) 85.39 85.39 85.39 .15 (.06, .24) .15 (.06, .24) .15 (.06, .24) .18 (.00) .18 (.00) .18 (.00) 2(431) 2(431) 2(431) 100.00 100.00 100.00 Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice .23 (.18, .27) .23 (.18, .27) .23 (.18, .27) .29 (.00) .29 (.00) .29 (.00) 2(1,794) 2(1,794) 2(1,794) 100.00 100.00 100.00 Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice .21 (.17, .26) .21 (.17, .26) .21 (.17, .26) .26 (.00) .26 (.00) .26 (.00) 2(1,883) 2(1,883) 2(1,883) 100.00 100.00 100.00 .18 .18 .18 1 (206) 1 (206) 1 (206) Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure .19 (.14, .23) .19 (.14, .23) .19 (.14, .23) .22 (.02) .22 (.02) .22 (.02) 10(1,994) 10(1,994) 10(1,994) 90.18 90.18 90.18 .25 (.20, .30) .25 (.20, .30) .25 (.20, .30) .30 (.06) .30 (.06) .30 (.06) 9(1,961) 9(1,961) 9(1,961) 63.06 63.06 63.06 Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice .13 (.09, .17) .13 (.09, .17) .13 (.09, .17) .15 (.02) .15 (.02) .15 (.02) 6 (2,633) 6 (2,633) 6 (2,633) 86.57 86.57 86.57 .20 (.14, .26) .20 (.14, .26) .20 (.14, .26) .25 (.00) .25 (.00) .25 (.00) 5 (903) 5 (903) 5 (903) 100.00 100.00 100.00 TR Withdrawal Withdrawal Withdrawal Negative reactions Negative reactions Negative reactions Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice -.36 (-.42, -.31) -.36 (-.42, -.31) -.36 (-.42, -.31) -.46 (.20) -.46 (.20) -.46 (.20) 39 (24,273) 39 (24,273) 39 (24,273) 4.12 4.12 4.12 -.27 (-.33, -.21) -.27 (-.33, -.21) -.27 (-.33, -.21) -.31 (.18) -.31 (.18) -.31 (.18) 27 (6,275) 27 (6,275) 27 (6,275) 13.37 13.37 13.37 Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions -.27 (-.32, -.21) -.27 (-.32, -.21) -.27 (-.32, -.21) -.34 (.14) -.34 (.14) -.34 (.14) 21 (7,344) 21 (7,344) 21 (7,344) 14.46 14.46 14.46 -.33 (-.39, -.26) -.33 (-.39, -.26) -.33 (-.39, -.26) -.38 (.12) -.38 (.12) -.38 (.12) 13 (3,563) 13 (3,563) 13 (3,563) 22.15 22.15 22.15 Process control Process control Process control Process control -.19 (-.27, -.10) -.19 (-.27, -.10) -.19 (-.27, -.10) -.24 (.14) -.24 (.14) -.24 (.14) 10(1,190) 10(1,190) 10(1,190) 39.94 39.94 39.94 -.23 (-.27, -.19) -.23 (-.27, -.19) -.23 (-.27, -.19) -.30 (.00) -.30 (.00) -.30 (.00) 3 (2,094) 3 (2,094) 3 (2,094) 100.00 100.00 100.00 Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria -.23 (-.30, -.16) -.23 (-.30, -.16) -.23 (-.30, -.16) -.29 (.07) -.29 (.07) -.29 (.07) 6(1,431) 6(1,431) 6(1,431) 50.87 50.87 50.87 -.28 (-.35, -.22) -.28 (-.35, -.22) -.28 (-.35, -.22) -.35 (.06) -.35 (.06) -.35 (.06) 5 (2,308) 5 (2,308) 5 (2,308) 34.76 34.76 34.76 Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice -.02 (-.13, .09) -.02 (-.13, .09) -.02 (-.13, .09) -.02 (.00) -.02 (.00) -.02 (.00) 2(316) 2(316) 2(316) 100.00 100.00 100.00 -.30 (-.35, -.26) -.30 (-.35, -.26) -.30 (-.35, -.26) -.35 (.04) -.35 (.04) -.35 (.04) 7 (2,707) 7 (2,707) 7 (2,707) 58.78 58.78 58.78 Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice -.21 (-.34, -.07) -.21 (-.34, -.07) -.21 (-.34, -.07) -.24 (.21) -.24 (.21) -.24 (.21) 8(1,692) 8(1,692) 8(1,692) 11.42 11.42 11.42 -.29 (-.34, -.24) -.29 (-.34, -.24) -.29 (-.34, -.24) -.33 (.06) -.33 (.06) -.33 (.06) 8(2,731) 8(2,731) 8(2,731) 45.25 45.25 45.25 Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure -.44 (-.56, -.32) -.44 (-.56, -.32) -.44 (-.56, -.32) -.55 (.19) -.55 (.19) -.55 (.19) 6 (14,392) 6 (14,392) 6 (14,392) 1.11 1.11 1.11 -.20 (-.35, -.05) -.20 (-.35, -.05) -.20 (-.35, -.05) -.22 (.21) -.22 (.21) -.22 (.21) 7(1,807) 7(1,807) 7(1,807) 9.28 9.28 9.28 Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice -.41 (-.46, -.37) -.41 (-.46, -.37) -.41 (-.46, -.37) -.50 (.12) -.50 (.12) -.50 (.12) 18(15,888) 18(15,888) 18(15,888) 7.45 7.45 7.45 -.26 (-.35, -.17) -.26 (-.35, -.17) -.26 (-.35, -.17) -.30 (.17) -.30 (.17) -.30 (.17) 13 (3,782) 13 (3,782) 13 (3,782) 12.27 12.27 12.27 TR Performance Performance Performance Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice Broadly defined procedural justice .30 (.21,-39) .30 (.21,-39) .30 (.21,-39) .36 (.29) .36 (.29) .36 (.29) 30 (8,317) 30 (8,317) 30 (8,317) 4.83 4.83 4.83 Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions Procedural fairness perceptions .30 (.17, .43) .30 (.17, .43) .30 (.17, .43) .36 (.33) .36 (.33) .36 (.33) 18 (6,925) 18 (6,925) 18 (6,925) 2.71 2.71 2.71 Process control Process control Process control Process control .14 (-.01,-29) .14 (-.01,-29) .14 (-.01,-29) .17 (.25) .17 (.25) .17 (.25) 8(1,002) 8(1,002) 8(1,002) 16.17 16.17 16.17 Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria Leventhal criteria .08 (-.05, .21) .08 (-.05, .21) .08 (-.05, .21) ■ 10(.ll) ■ 10(.ll) ■ 10(.ll) 3 (501) 3 (501) 3 (501) 44.34 44.34 44.34 Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice Interpersonal justice .03 (-.14, .20) .03 (-.14, .20) .03 (-.14, .20) .03 (.11) .03 (.11) .03 (.11) 2 (389) 2 (389) 2 (389) 34.78 34.78 34.78 Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice Informational justice .11 (.00, .22) .11 (.00, .22) .11 (.00, .22) .13 (.11) .13 (.11) .13 (.11) 4(1,036) 4(1,036) 4(1,036) 29.79 29.79 29.79 Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure Indirect combination measure .23 (.12, .35) .23 (.12, .35) .23 (.12, .35) .27 (.00) .27 (.00) .27 (.00) 7 (1,084) 7 (1,084) 7 (1,084) 23.83 23.83 23.83 Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice Distributive justice .13 (.03, .22) .13 (.03, .22) .13 (.03, .22) .15 (.18) .15 (.18) .15 (.18) 13(2,294) . 13(2,294) . 13(2,294) . 18.43 18.43 18.43 Note. r = uncorrected population correlation; CI = confidence interval around uncorrected population correlation; r. = corrected population correlation; SDr = standard deviation of corrected population correlation; Note. r = uncorrected population correlation; CI = confidence interval around uncorrected population correlation; r. = corrected population correlation; SDr = standard deviation of corrected population correlation; к = number of studies; Vart = percentage of variance in rc explained by study artifacts; OCBs = organizational citizenship behaviors. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 437 437 Table 6 Table 6 Table 6 Table 6 Unique Effects of Procedural, Interpersonal, Informational, and Distributive Justice on Outcome Variables Unique Effects of Procedural, Interpersonal, Informational, and Distributive Justice on Outcome Variables Outcome variable Outcome variable Outcome variable Outcome variable Justice dimension Justice dimension Justice dimension Total R2 Total R2 Total R2 TR Procedural Procedural Procedural Interpersonal Interpersonal Interpersonal Informational Informational Informational Distributive Distributive Distributive Outcome satisfaction (N = 1,792) Outcome satisfaction (N = 1,792) Outcome satisfaction (N = 1,792) Outcome satisfaction (N = 1,792) ß ß ß ß .17* .17* .17* -.08* -.08* -.08* .02 .02 .02 .54* .54* .54* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .02* .02* .02* .00* .00* .00* .00 .00 .00 .18* .18* .18* .39* .39* .39* Job satisfaction (N = 4,039) Job satisfaction (N = 4,039) Job satisfaction (N = 4,039) Job satisfaction (N = 4,039) ß ß ß ß .48* .48* .48* -.09* -.09* -.09* .13* .13* .13* .26* .26* .26* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .11* .11* .11* .01* .01* .01* .01* .01* .01* .04* .04* .04* .45* .45* .45* Organizational commitment (N = 4,582) Organizational commitment (N = 4,582) Organizational commitment (N = 4,582) Organizational commitment (N = 4,582) ß ß ß ß .42* .42* .42* -.18* -.18* -.18* .07* .07* .07* .31* .31* .31* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .09* .09* .09* .02* .02* .02* .00* .00* .00* .06* .06* .06* .35* .35* .35* Evaluation of authority: Agent-referenced (N = 4,517) Evaluation of authority: Agent-referenced (N = 4,517) Evaluation of authority: Agent-referenced (N = 4,517) Evaluation of authority: Agent-referenced (N = 4,517) ß ß ß ß . .01 .01 .01 .27* .27* .27* .32* .32* .32* .32* .32* .32* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .00 .00 .00 .04* .04* .04* .05* .05* .05* .06* .06* .06* .57* .57* .57* Evaluation of authority: System-referenced (N = 2,114) Evaluation of authority: System-referenced (N = 2,114) Evaluation of authority: System-referenced (N = 2,114) Evaluation of authority: System-referenced (N = 2,114) ß ß ß ß .30* .30* .30* -.19* -.19* -.19* .44* .44* .44* .11* .11* .11* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .04* .04* .04* .02* .02* .02* .10* .10* .10* .01* .01* .01* .31* .31* .31* Trust (N = 1,711) Trust (N = 1,711) Trust (N = 1,711) Trust (N = 1,711) ß ß ß ß Unique R2 Unique R2 .31* .31* .31* .05* .05* .21* .21* .21* .03* .03* .30* .30* .30* .05* .05* .45* .45* .45* OCBs: Individual referenced (N = 3,192) OCBs: Individual referenced (N = 3,192) OCBs: Individual referenced (N = 3,192) OCBs: Individual referenced (N = 3,192) ß ß ß ß .06* .06* .06* .19* .19* .19* .11* .11* .11* -.01* -.01* -.01* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .00* .00* .00* .02* .02* .02* .01* .01* .01* .00 .00 .00 .09* .09* .09* OCBs: Organization referenced (N = 782) OCBs: Organization referenced (N = 782) OCBs: Organization referenced (N = 782) OCBs: Organization referenced (N = 782) ß ß ß ß Unique R2 Unique R2 .12* .12* .12* .01* .01* .11* .11* .11* .01* .01* .12* .12* .12* .01* .01* .08* .08* .08* Performance (N = 1,855) Performance (N = 1,855) Performance (N = 1,855) Performance (N = 1,855) ß ß ß ß .56* .56* .56* -.20* -.20* -.20* .07* .07* .07* -.07 -.07 -.07 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .15* .15* .15* .02* .02* .02* .00* .00* .00* .00* .00* .00* .19* .19* .19* Withdrawal (N = 1,919) Withdrawal (N = 1,919) Withdrawal (N = 1,919) Withdrawal (N = 1,919) ß ß ß ß -.10* -.10* -.10* .23* .23* .23* -.21* -.21* -.21* -.51* -.51* -.51* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .01* .01* .01* .02* .02* .02* .02* .02* .02* .16* .16* .16* .33* .33* .33* Negative reactions (N = 4,039) Negative reactions (N = 4,039) Negative reactions (N = 4,039) Negative reactions (N = 4,039) ß ß ß ß -.06* -.06* -.06* -.18* -.18* -.18* -.12* -.12* -.12* -.14* -.14* -.14* Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 Unique R2 .00* .00* .00* .02* .02* .02* .01* .01* .01* .01* .01* .01* .16* .16* .16* Note. Sample sizes are based on the harmonic mean of the sample sizes for the correlations among the justice and outcome variables. The procedural justice variable includes process control, Leventhal criteria, and procedural fairness perceptions. OCBs = organizational citizenship behaviors. Note. Sample sizes are based on the harmonic mean of the sample sizes for the correlations among the justice and outcome variables. The procedural justice variable includes process control, Leventhal criteria, and procedural fairness perceptions. OCBs = organizational citizenship behaviors. * p < .05. * p < .05. model, which predicts that distributive justice will have stronger effects than the other justice dimensions. This model was sup­ported for outcome satisfaction and withdrawal but not for any of the other nine outcomes. model, which predicts that distributive justice will have stronger effects than the other justice dimensions. This model was sup­ported for outcome satisfaction and withdrawal but not for any of the other nine outcomes. The two-factor model predicts that procedural justice will have stronger effects than distributive justice on system-referenced vari­ables but weaker effects than distributive justice on person- referenced variables. This model seemed to receive support only for person-referenced and system-referenced attitudes such as out­come satisfaction, job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and system-referenced evaluation of authority. The two-factor model’s predictions were not supported for more behavioral va The two-factor model predicts that procedural justice will have stronger effects than distributive justice on system-referenced vari­ables but weaker effects than distributive justice on person- referenced variables. This model seemed to receive support only for person-referenced and system-referenced attitudes such as out­come satisfaction, job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and system-referenced evaluation of authority. The two-factor model’s predictions were not supported for more behavioral va The agent-system model predicts that interpersonal or informa­tional justice will have stronger effects than procedural justice on agent-referenced variables but weaker effects than procedural jus­tice on system-referenced variables. This model was supported for The agent-system model predicts that interpersonal or informa­tional justice will have stronger effects than procedural justice on agent-referenced variables but weaker effects than procedural jus­tice on system-referenced variables. This model was supported for agent-referenced outcomes, including agent-referenced evaluation of authority and OCBIs, but not for trust, which was more related to procedural and distributive justice. The agent-system model was also supported for job satisfaction, organizational commit­ment, and performance. The model actually seems to underesti­mate the importance of interpersonal or informational justice for behavioral variables. Interpersonal or informational justice was a strong predictor of OCBOs, withdrawal, and negative reactions agent-referenced outcomes, including agent-referenced evaluation of authority and OCBIs, but not for trust, which was more related to procedural and distributive justice. The agent-system model was also supported for job satisfaction, organizational commit­ment, and performance. The model actually seems to underesti­mate the importance of interpersonal or informational justice for behavioral variables. Interpersonal or informational justice was a strong predictor of OCBOs, withdrawal, and negative reactions Implications for the Organizational Justice Literature Implications for the Organizational Justice Literature The conceptualization, measurement, and analysis of organiza­tional justice depend in large part on a given study’s research question, as well as the sample or setting used to examine it. Nonetheless, the results of our review have some broad, general implications for the justice literature as a whole. The conceptualization, measurement, and analysis of organiza­tional justice depend in large part on a given study’s research question, as well as the sample or setting used to examine it. Nonetheless, the results of our review have some broad, general implications for the justice literature as a whole. Distinctiveness of justice dimensions. The construct discrimi­nation results suggest that procedural, interpersonal, and informa­tional justice are distinct constructs that can be empirically Distinctiveness of justice dimensions. The construct discrimi­nation results suggest that procedural, interpersonal, and informa­tional justice are distinct constructs that can be empirically distin- 438 438 438 438 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG guished from one another. We would therefore call for a moratorium on indirect combination measures that combine the three justice dimensions into a single variable. Thirty-five of the studies included in our review used such measures (e.g., Brockner guished from one another. We would therefore call for a moratorium on indirect combination measures that combine the three justice dimensions into a single variable. Thirty-five of the studies included in our review used such measures (e.g., Brockner et al., 1995, 1997; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997). Our review showed that procedural, interpersonal, and informa­tional justice have different correlates, and measuring the three separately allows for further differences among the dimensions to be examined. Measurement of justice dimensions. It is also critical that researchers devote more care and effort to the measurement of justice dimensions. Some of the articles we reviewed used mea­sures that assessed, among supervisors, granting subordinates voice, treating subordinates consistently, suppressing biases, being respectful, and providing explanations. Such measures were la­beled “interactional justice” (e.g., Moorman, 1991; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997), even though only the latter two are included in Measurement of justice dimensions. It is also critical that researchers devote more care and effort to the measurement of justice dimensions. Some of the articles we reviewed used mea­sures that assessed, among supervisors, granting subordinates voice, treating subordinates consistently, suppressing biases, being respectful, and providing explanations. Such measures were la­beled “interactional justice” (e.g., Moorman, 1991; Skarlicki & Latham, 1997), even though only the latter two are included in Bies and Moag’s (1986) construct. Authors may have included such items on the basis of Folger and Bies’s (1989) article on “managerial responsibilities” for procedural justice, which listed seven principles (process control, bias suppression, consistency, feedback, justification, truthfulness, and courtesy) managers should use to promote procedural justice. Not surprisingly, re­searchers using such measures have been forced to collapse the interactional justice measure with their procedural justice measure as a Distinguishing justice content from justice source. Relatedly, future research should seek to separate the effects of justice con­tent from the effects of justice source. Procedural justice can be a function of an organization, as when a formalized decision-making system provides process control or consistency as a result of the way in which it is structured (e.g., Adams-Roy & Barling, 1998; Distinguishing justice content from justice source. Relatedly, future research should seek to separate the effects of justice con­tent from the effects of justice source. Procedural justice can be a function of an organization, as when a formalized decision-making system provides process control or consistency as a result of the way in which it is structured (e.g., Adams-Roy & Barling, 1998; Ehlen, Magner, & Welker, 1999; Skarlicki et al., 1999). Procedural justice can also be a function of a decision-making agent, as when a manager takes steps to involve subordinates in decisions or treat subordinates consistently (e.g., Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky, Folger, & Cropanzano, 1987; Korsgaard, Schweiger, & Sapienza, 1995). Likewise, informational and interpersonal justice can be fostered through organizational policies and initiatives (e.g., Konovsky & Folger, 1991; Mansour-Cole & Scott, 1998; Tansky, 1993) or through the actions of a decision-making agent (e.g., Bies, Shapiro, & Cummings, 1988; Greenberg, 1994; Moorman, 1991). It is difficult to draw conclusions from studies comparing the effects of organization-originating procedural justice with agent-originating interpersonal or informational justice because source and content are confounded. Fortunately, recent research is beginning to examine this important issue by crossing justice source with justice content to assess their joint effects (Blader & Tyler, 2000; Byrne & Cropanzano, 2000; Masterson, Bartol, & Moye, 2000). Including multiple justice dimensions in single studies. Our results also clearly show that researchers can explain more out­come variance by including multiple justice dimensions within single studies, although there are some diminishing returns asso­ciated with this strategy. Our proactive research results showed that distributive justice, procedural justice (in terms of Leventhal criteria), interpersonal justice, and, to a lesser extent, informational justice each contribute uniquely to the creation of f Including multiple justice dimensions in single studies. Our results also clearly show that researchers can explain more out­come variance by including multiple justice dimensions within single studies, although there are some diminishing returns asso­ciated with this strategy. Our proactive research results showed that distributive justice, procedural justice (in terms of Leventhal criteria), interpersonal justice, and, to a lesser extent, informational justice each contribute uniquely to the creation of f Examining interactions among justice dimensions. In addition to explaining more outcome variance, including multiple dimen­sions will also allow for the testing of interaction effects. The interaction between procedural and distributive justice has been perhaps the most robust finding in the justice literature (Brockner & Examining interactions among justice dimensions. In addition to explaining more outcome variance, including multiple dimen­sions will also allow for the testing of interaction effects. The interaction between procedural and distributive justice has been perhaps the most robust finding in the justice literature (Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996). Skarlicki and Folger (1997) further showed that procedural and interactional justice interact in predicting ORBs. These authors also found support for three-way interactions among procedural, interactional, and distributive justice. Such complex relationships do not map neatly onto reactive models such as the two-factor or agent-system model. However, the fact that moderators were present in almost all of the justice-outcome relationships suggests that more complex relationships may be the key to imp Gaps in the existing literature. A final implication of our results resides in the gaps revealed by this review. A scan of Table 5 highlights areas where more research ought to be done. In terms of sheer number of studies, we note that procedural justice is much better represented in studies of satisfaction, commitment, evaluation of authorities, withdrawal, and negative reactions and relatively underrepresented in studies of performance, OCBs, and trust. We also note that interpersonal and informational ju Gaps in the existing literature. A final implication of our results resides in the gaps revealed by this review. A scan of Table 5 highlights areas where more research ought to be done. In terms of sheer number of studies, we note that procedural justice is much better represented in studies of satisfaction, commitment, evaluation of authorities, withdrawal, and negative reactions and relatively underrepresented in studies of performance, OCBs, and trust. We also note that interpersonal and informational ju Limitations Limitations This review has some limitations that should be noted. First, as in the primary studies on which our review is based, many of the variables were assessed with self-report measures. Thus, many of the relationships may be inflated because of same source bias. Second, any meta-analysis is subject to a variety of judgment calls This review has some limitations that should be noted. First, as in the primary studies on which our review is based, many of the variables were assessed with self-report measures. Thus, many of the relationships may be inflated because of same source bias. Second, any meta-analysis is subject to a variety of judgment calls META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 439 439 (Wanous, Sullivan, & (Wanous, Sullivan, & Malinak, 1989). Although we performed all coding in author dyads, it is possible that some of these judgment calls affected our results. Finally, the fact that meta-analysis re­quires the reporting of zero-order results meant that several im­portant justice articles could not be included in our review. In particular, many of Tyler’s examinations of the relational model of justice had to be omitted (e.g., Tyler, 1989, 1994; Tyler et al., 1996). Conclusion Conclusion In a review of the organizational justice literature, Greenberg (1993a) suggested that the field was in a state of “intellectual adolescence.” This adolescence, though marked by many advance­ments, is also characterized by “stumbling awkwardness” due to underdeveloped research agendas and the absence of underlying theory (Greenberg, 1993a, p. 139). In an earlier review, Greenberg (1990b) echoed these sentiments, noting that the field was years away from the final stage of Reichers and Schneider’s (1990) con In a review of the organizational justice literature, Greenberg (1993a) suggested that the field was in a state of “intellectual adolescence.” This adolescence, though marked by many advance­ments, is also characterized by “stumbling awkwardness” due to underdeveloped research agendas and the absence of underlying theory (Greenberg, 1993a, p. 139). In an earlier review, Greenberg (1990b) echoed these sentiments, noting that the field was years away from the final stage of Reichers and Schneider’s (1990) con , References , References References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis. References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis. Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267-299). New York: Academic Press. Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267-299). New York: Academic Press. *Adams-Roy, J., & Barling, J. (1998). Predicting the decision to confront or report sexual harassment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 329-336. *Adams-Roy, J., & Barling, J. (1998). Predicting the decision to confront or report sexual harassment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 329-336. Alexander, S., & Alexander, S., & Ruderman, Μ. (1987). The role of procedural and distributive justice in organizational behavior. Social Justice Re­search, 1, 177-198. Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 1-18. Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 1-18. *Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, *Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. (1989). The influence of social compar­isons on perceptions of procedural fairness. Journal of Business and Psychology, 4, 129-138. *Aquino, K. (1995). Relationships among pay inequity, perceptions of procedural justice, and organizational citizenship. Employee Responsi­bilities and Rights Journal, 8, 21-33. *Aquino, K. (1995). Relationships among pay inequity, perceptions of procedural justice, and organizational citizenship. Employee Responsi­bilities and Rights Journal, 8, 21-33. »Aquino, K., Griffeth, »Aquino, K., Griffeth, R. W., Allen, D. G., & Hom, P. W. (1997). Integrating justice constructs into the turnover process: A test of a referent cognitions model. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 1208­ 1227. »Armstrong-Stassen, Μ. (1998). The effect of gender and organizational level on how survivors appraise and cope with organizational downsiz­ing. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 34, 125-142. »Armstrong-Stassen, Μ. (1998). The effect of gender and organizational level on how survivors appraise and cope with organizational downsiz­ing. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 34, 125-142. »Austin, W., McGinn, N. C., & Susmilch, C. (1980). Internal standards revisited: Effects of social comparisons and expectancies on judgments of fairness and satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol­ogy, 16, 426-441. »Austin, W., McGinn, N. C., & Susmilch, C. (1980). Internal standards revisited: Effects of social comparisons and expectancies on judgments of fairness and satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol­ogy, 16, 426-441. »Ball, G. A., Trevino, L. K. & Sims, »Ball, G. A., Trevino, L. K. & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1993). Justice and organizational punishment: Attitudinal outcomes of disciplinary events. Social Justice Research, 6, 39-67. »Ball, G. A., Trevino, L. K., & Sims, »Ball, G. A., Trevino, L. K., & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1994). Just and unjust punishment: Influences on subordinate performance and citizenship. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 299-322. »Barclay, »Barclay, J. H., & Harland, L. K. (1995). Peer performance appraisals: The impact of rater competence, rater location, and rating correctability on fairness perceptions. Group and Organization Management, 20, 39-60. »Barling, J., & Phillips, Μ. (1993). Interactional, formal and distributive justice in the workplace: An exploratory study. Journal of Psychology, 127, 649-656. »Barling, J., & Phillips, Μ. (1993). Interactional, formal and distributive justice in the workplace: An exploratory study. Journal of Psychology, 127, 649-656. Bazerman, Μ. H., Bazerman, Μ. H., Schroth, H„ Shah, P., Diekmann, К., & Tenbrunsel, A. (1994). The inconsistent role of comparison others and procedural jus­tice in reactions to hypothetical job descriptions; Implications for job acceptance decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 326-352. »Bennett, »Bennett, N., Martin, C. L., Bies, R. J., & Brockner, J. (1995). Coping with a layoff: A longitudinal study of victims. Journal of Management, 21, 1025-1040. »Bennett, R. J. (1998). Taking the sting out of the whip: Reactions to consistent punishment for unethical behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 248-262. »Bennett, R. J. (1998). Taking the sting out of the whip: Reactions to consistent punishment for unethical behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 248-262. »Bies, »Bies, R. J. (1987). Beyond “voice”: The influence of decision-maker justification and sincerity on procedural fairness judgments. Represen­tative Research in Social Psychology, 17, 3-14. »Bies, »Bies, R. J., Martin, C. L., & Brockner, J. (1993). Just laid off, but still a “good citizen?” Only if the process is fair. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 6, 227-238. Bies, Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. F. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness. In R. J. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiations in organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 43-55). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. »Bies, »Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (1987). Interactional fairness judgments: The influence of causal accounts. Social Justice Research, 1, 199-218. »Bies, »Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (1988). Voice and justification: Their influence on procedural fairness judgments. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 676-685. »Bies, »Bies, R. J., Shapiro, D. L., & Cummings, L. L. (1988). Causal accounts and managing organizational conflict: Is it enough to say it’s not my fault? Communication Research, 15, 381-399. Blader, Blader, S. L., & Tyler, T. R. (2000, August). A four-component model of procedural justice: What makes a process fair in work settings? Paper presented at the 59th Annual Meeting of the National Academy of Management, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley. Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley. »Bobocel, D. R., Agar, S. E., Meyer, J. P., & Irving, P. G. (1998). Managerial accounts and fairness perceptions in conflict resolution: Differentiating the effects of minimizing responsibility and providing justification. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 20, 133-143. Managerial accounts and fairness perceptions in conflict resolution: Differentiating the effects of minimizing responsibility and providing justification. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 20, 133-143. »Bobocel, »Bobocel, D. R., & Farrell, A. C. (1996). Sex-based promotion decisions and interactional fairness: Investigating the influence of managerial accounts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 22-35. Borman, W. C. (1991). Job behavior, performance, and effectiveness. In Borman, W. C. (1991). Job behavior, performance, and effectiveness. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 271-326). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. »Brockner, »Brockner, J., DeWitt, R. L., Grover, S., & Reed, T. (1990). When it is especially important to explain why: Factors affecting the relationship between managers’ explanations of a layoff and survivors’ reactions to the layoff. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 389-407. »Brockner, »Brockner, J., Konovsky, Μ., Cooper-Schneider, R., Folger, R., Martin, C., & Bies, R. J. (1994). Interactive effects of procedural justice and 440 440 440 440 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG outcome negativity on victims and survivors of job loss. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 397-409. outcome negativity on victims and survivors of job loss. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 397-409. *Brockner, L, Siegel, P. A., Daly, J. P., & Martin, C. (1997). When trust matters: The moderating effect of outcome favorability. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 558-583. *Brockner, L, Siegel, P. A., Daly, J. P., & Martin, C. (1997). When trust matters: The moderating effect of outcome favorability. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 558-583. *Brockner, J., Tyler, T. Μ., & Cooper-Schneider, R. (1992). The influence of prior commitment to an institution on reactions to perceived unfair­ness: The higher they are, the harder they fall. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 241-261. *Brockner, J., Tyler, T. Μ., & Cooper-Schneider, R. (1992). The influence of prior commitment to an institution on reactions to perceived unfair­ness: The higher they are, the harder they fall. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 241-261. Brockner, J., & Brockner, J., & Wiesenfeld, B. M. (1996). An integrative framework for explaining reactions to decisions: Interactive effects of outcomes and procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 189-208. *Brockner, J., *Brockner, J., Wiesenfeld, В. Μ., & Martin, C. L. (1995). Decision frame, procedural justice, and survivors’ reactions to job layoffs. Organiza­tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63, 59-68. *Brockner, J., *Brockner, J., Wiesenfeld, В. Μ., Reed, T., Grover, S., & Martin, C. (1993). Interactive effect of job content and context on the reactions of layoff survivors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 187-197. Byrne, Byrne, Z. S., & Cropanzano, R. S. (2000, April). To which do I attribute this fairness? Differential effects of multi-foci justice on organizational work behaviors. Paper presented at the 15th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, New Orleans, LA. *Cobb, A. T., & Frey, F. Μ. (1996). The effects of leader fairness and pay outcomes on superior/subordinate relations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26. 1401-1426. *Cobb, A. T., & Frey, F. Μ. (1996). The effects of leader fairness and pay outcomes on superior/subordinate relations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26. 1401-1426. ♦Cobb, A. T., Vest. Μ., & Hills, F. (1997). Who delivers justice? Source perceptions of procedural fairness. Journal of Applied Social Psychol­ogy, 27, 1021-1040. ♦Cobb, A. T., Vest. Μ., & Hills, F. (1997). Who delivers justice? Source perceptions of procedural fairness. Journal of Applied Social Psychol­ogy, 27, 1021-1040. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Eribaum. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Eribaum. *Colquitt, J. A. (1999). The impact of procedural justice in teams: An analysis of task, team, and member moderators. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing. *Colquitt, J. A. (1999). The impact of procedural justice in teams: An analysis of task, team, and member moderators. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing. *Colquitt, J. A. (2000). Justice in teams: An analysis of antecedents and consequences. Unpublished manuscript. *Colquitt, J. A. (2000). Justice in teams: An analysis of antecedents and consequences. Unpublished manuscript. *Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386-400. *Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386-400. *Colquitt, J. A., & Chertkoff, J. M. (1996, April). Explaining injustice: The interactive effect of explanation and outcome on fairness perceptions and task motivation. Paper presented at the 11th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA. *Colquitt, J. A., & Chertkoff, J. M. (1996, April). Explaining injustice: The interactive effect of explanation and outcome on fairness perceptions and task motivation. Paper presented at the 11th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA. Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 678-707. Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 678-707. *Conlon, *Conlon, D. E. (1993). Some tests of the self-interest and group value models of procedural justice: Evidence from an organizational appeal procedure. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 1109-1124. *Conlon, D. E., & *Conlon, D. E., & Fasolo, P. M. (1990). Influence of speed of third-party intervention and outcome on negotiator and constituent fairness judg­ments. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 833-846. *Conlon, D. E., & Lynch, *Conlon, D. E., & Lynch, P. D. (1997, June). The role of contracts and explanations on justice judgments and agent compensation. Paper pre­sented at the 10th Annual Conference of the International Association for Conflict Management, Bonn, Germany. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Murray, N. M. (1996). Customer perceptions of corpo­rate responses to product complaints: The role of explanations. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 1040-1056. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Murray, N. M. (1996). Customer perceptions of corpo­rate responses to product complaints: The role of explanations. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 1040-1056. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Parks, J. Μ. (1999). Effects of resources and reward allocation rules on justice judgments. Unpublished manuscript. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Parks, J. Μ. (1999). Effects of resources and reward allocation rules on justice judgments. Unpublished manuscript. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Ross, W. H. (1993). The effects of partisan third parties ♦Conlon, D. E., & Ross, W. H. (1993). The effects of partisan third parties on negotiator behavior and outcome perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 280-290. on negotiator behavior and outcome perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 280-290. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Ross, W. H. (1997). Appearances do count: The effects of outcomes and explanations on disputant fairness judgments and supervisory evaluations. International Journal of Conflict Manage­ment, 8, 5-31. ♦Conlon, D. E., & Ross, W. H. (1997). Appearances do count: The effects of outcomes and explanations on disputant fairness judgments and supervisory evaluations. International Journal of Conflict Manage­ment, 8, 5-31. Cropanzano, R., & Ambrose, Cropanzano, R., & Ambrose, M. L. (2001). Procedural and distributive justice are more similar than you think: A monistic perspective and a research agenda. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 119-151). Lexington, MA: New Lexington Press. Cropanzano, R., & Greenberg, J. (1997). Progress in organizational justice: Tunneling through the maze. In C. Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 317-372). New York: Wiley. Cropanzano, R., & Greenberg, J. (1997). Progress in organizational justice: Tunneling through the maze. In C. Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 317-372). New York: Wiley. ♦Cropanzano, R., & Konovsky, M. A. (1996). Resolving the justice di­lemma by improving the outcomes: The case of employee drug screen­ing. Journal of Business and Psychology, II, 239-263. ♦Cropanzano, R., & Konovsky, M. A. (1996). Resolving the justice di­lemma by improving the outcomes: The case of employee drug screen­ing. Journal of Business and Psychology, II, 239-263. ♦Cropanzano, R., & Prehar, C. A. (1999, April). Using social exchange theory to distinguish procedural from interactional justice. Paper pre­sented at the 14th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Atlanta, GA. ♦Cropanzano, R., & Prehar, C. A. (1999, April). Using social exchange theory to distinguish procedural from interactional justice. Paper pre­sented at the 14th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Atlanta, GA. Cudeck, R. (1989). Analysis of correlation matrices using covariance structure models. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 317-327. Cudeck, R. (1989). Analysis of correlation matrices using covariance structure models. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 317-327. ♦Dailey, R. C., & Kirk, D. J. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as antecedents of job dissatisfaction and intent to turnover. Human Rela­tions, 45, 305-317. ♦Dailey, R. C., & Kirk, D. J. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as antecedents of job dissatisfaction and intent to turnover. Human Rela­tions, 45, 305-317. ♦Daly, J. P. (1995). Explaining changes to employees: The influence of justifications and change outcomes on employees’ fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 31, 415-428. ♦Daly, J. P. (1995). Explaining changes to employees: The influence of justifications and change outcomes on employees’ fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 31, 415-428. ♦Daly, J. P., & Geyer, ♦Daly, J. P., & Geyer, P. D. (1994). The role of fairness in implementing large-scale change: Employee evaluations of process and outcome in seven facility relocations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 623­ 638. ♦Davy, J. A., Kinicki, A. J., & ♦Davy, J. A., Kinicki, A. J., & Scheck, C. L. (1991). Developing and testing a model of survivor responses to layoffs. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 38, 302-317. Deutsch, Μ. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31, 137-150. Deutsch, Μ. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31, 137-150. ♦DeWitt, R. L., Trevino, L. K., & ♦DeWitt, R. L., Trevino, L. K., & Mollica, K. A. (1998). The influence of eligibility on employees’ reactions to voluntary workforce reductions. Journal of Management, 24, 593-613. ♦Dittrich, J. E., & Carrell, Μ. R. (1979). Organizational equity perceptions, employee job satisfaction, and department absence and turnover rates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 24, 29-40. ♦Dittrich, J. E., & Carrell, Μ. R. (1979). Organizational equity perceptions, employee job satisfaction, and department absence and turnover rates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 24, 29-40. ♦Donovan, M. A., Drasgow, F., & Munson, L. J. (1998). The Perceptions of Fair Interpersonal Treatment Scale: Development and validation of a measure of interpersonal treatment in the workplace. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 683-692. ♦Donovan, M. A., Drasgow, F., & Munson, L. J. (1998). The Perceptions of Fair Interpersonal Treatment Scale: Development and validation of a measure of interpersonal treatment in the workplace. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 683-692. ♦Dulebohn, J. H., & Ferris, G. R. (1999). The role of influence tactics in perceptions of performance evaluations’ fairness. Academy of Manage­ment Journal, 42, 288-303. ♦Dulebohn, J. H., & Ferris, G. R. (1999). The role of influence tactics in perceptions of performance evaluations’ fairness. Academy of Manage­ment Journal, 42, 288-303. ♦Dulebohn, J. H., & Martocchio, J. J. (1998). Employee perceptions of the fairness of work group incentive pay plans. Journal of Management, 24, 469-488. ♦Dulebohn, J. H., & Martocchio, J. J. (1998). Employee perceptions of the fairness of work group incentive pay plans. Journal of Management, 24, 469-488. ♦Earley, P. C., & Lind, E. A. (1987). Procedural justice and participation in task selection: The role of control in mediating justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1148-1160. ♦Earley, P. C., & Lind, E. A. (1987). Procedural justice and participation in task selection: The role of control in mediating justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1148-1160. ♦Ehlen, ♦Ehlen, C. R., Magner, N. R., & Welker, R. B. (1999). Testing for interactive effects of outcome favourability and procedural fairness on members’ reactions towards a voluntary professional organization. Jour­nal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 72, 147-161. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 441 441 ♦Farh, J., Earley, P. C., & Lin, S. (1997). Impetus for action: A cultural analysis of justice and organizational citizenship behavior in Chinese society. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 421-444. ♦Farh, J., Earley, P. C., & Lin, S. (1997). Impetus for action: A cultural analysis of justice and organizational citizenship behavior in Chinese society. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 421-444. Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: Combined impact of “voice” and improvement on experienced inequity Journal of Person­ality and Social Psychology, 35, 108-119. Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: Combined impact of “voice” and improvement on experienced inequity Journal of Person­ality and Social Psychology, 35, 108-119. Folger, R. (1987). Distributive and procedural justice in the workplace. Social Justice Research, 1, 143-159. Folger, R. (1987). Distributive and procedural justice in the workplace. Social Justice Research, 1, 143-159. Folger, R., & Folger, R., & Bies, R. J. (1989). Managerial responsibilities and procedural justice. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 2, 79-89. Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Organizational justice and human resource management. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Organizational justice and human resource management. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. *Folger, R., & Konovsky, Μ. A. (1989). Effects of procedural and distrib­utive justice on reactions to pay raise decisions. Academy of Manage­ment Journal, 32, 115-130. *Folger, R., & Konovsky, Μ. A. (1989). Effects of procedural and distrib­utive justice on reactions to pay raise decisions. Academy of Manage­ment Journal, 32, 115-130. ♦Folger, R„ & Martin, C. (1986). Relative deprivation and referent cog­nitions: Distributive and procedural justice effects. Journal of Experi­mental Social Psychology, 22, 531-546. ♦Folger, R„ & Martin, C. (1986). Relative deprivation and referent cog­nitions: Distributive and procedural justice effects. Journal of Experi­mental Social Psychology, 22, 531-546. *Folger, R., Rosenfield, D., Grove, J., & Corkran, L. (1979). Effects of “voice” and peer opinions and responses to inequity. Journal of Person­ality and Social Psychology, 37, 2253-2261. *Folger, R., Rosenfield, D., Grove, J., & Corkran, L. (1979). Effects of “voice” and peer opinions and responses to inequity. Journal of Person­ality and Social Psychology, 37, 2253-2261. ♦Folger, ♦Folger, R., Rosenfield, D., Hays, R., & Grove, R. (1978). Justice vs. justification effects on productivity: Reconciling equity and dissonance findings. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 22, 465-473. ♦Folger, ♦Folger, R., & Skarlicki, D. P. (1998). When tough times make tough bosses: Managerial distancing as a function of layoff blame. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 79-87. *Frost, S. E. (Ed.). (1972). Masterworks of philosophy (Vol. 1, pp. *Frost, S. E. (Ed.). (1972). Masterworks of philosophy (Vol. 1, pp. 135­ 140). Boston: McGraw-Hill. *Fryxell, G. E., & Gordon, Μ. E. (1989). Workplace justice and job satisfaction as predictors of satisfaction with union and management. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 851-866. *Fryxell, G. E., & Gordon, Μ. E. (1989). Workplace justice and job satisfaction as predictors of satisfaction with union and management. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 851-866. *Gellatly, I. R. (1995). Individual and group determinants of employee absenteeism: Test of a causal model. Journal of Organizational Behav­ior, 16, 469-485. *Gellatly, I. R. (1995). Individual and group determinants of employee absenteeism: Test of a causal model. Journal of Organizational Behav­ior, 16, 469-485. ♦George, J. Μ. (1991). State or trait: Effects of positive mood on prosocial behaviors at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299-307. ♦George, J. Μ. (1991). State or trait: Effects of positive mood on prosocial behaviors at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299-307. ♦Giacobbe-Miller, J. (1995). A test of the group values and control models of procedural justice from the competing perspectives of labor and management. Personnel Psychology, 48, 115-142. ♦Giacobbe-Miller, J. (1995). A test of the group values and control models of procedural justice from the competing perspectives of labor and management. Personnel Psychology, 48, 115-142. ♦Giles, W. F., Findley, H. Μ., & Field, H. S. (1997). Procedural fairness in performance appraisal: Beyond the review session. Journal of Business and Psychology, 11, 493-506. ♦Giles, W. F., Findley, H. Μ., & Field, H. S. (1997). Procedural fairness in performance appraisal: Beyond the review session. Journal of Business and Psychology, 11, 493-506. ♦Gilliland, S. W. (1994). Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions to a selection system. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 691-701. ♦Gilliland, S. W. (1994). Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions to a selection system. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 691-701. ♦Gilliland, S. W., & ♦Gilliland, S. W., & Beckstein, В. A. (1996). Procedural and distributive justice in the editorial review process. Personnel Psychology, 49, 669­ 691. ♦Gilliland, S. W., Benson, L., & Schepers, D. H. (1998). A rejection threshold in justice evaluations: Effects on judgment and decision­making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 113-131. ♦Gilliland, S. W., Benson, L., & Schepers, D. H. (1998). A rejection threshold in justice evaluations: Effects on judgment and decision­making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 113-131. ♦Gordon, M. E., & Bowlby, R. L. (1988). Propositions about grievance settlements: Finally, consultation with grievants. Personnel Psychol­ogy, 41, 107-123. ♦Gordon, M. E., & Bowlby, R. L. (1988). Propositions about grievance settlements: Finally, consultation with grievants. Personnel Psychol­ogy, 41, 107-123. ♦Gordon, Μ. E., & Fryxell, G. E. (1989). Voluntariness of association as a moderator of the importance of procedural and distributive justice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 993-1009. ♦Gordon, Μ. E., & Fryxell, G. E. (1989). Voluntariness of association as a moderator of the importance of procedural and distributive justice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 993-1009. Graen, G. B., & Graen, G. B., & Scandura, T. (1987). Toward a psychology of dyadic organizing. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 175-208). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. organizational behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 175-208). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. ♦Greenberg, J. (1987a). Reactions to procedural injustice in payment distributions: Do the means justify the ends? Journal of Applied Psy­chology, 72, 55-61. ♦Greenberg, J. (1987a). Reactions to procedural injustice in payment distributions: Do the means justify the ends? Journal of Applied Psy­chology, 72, 55-61. Greenberg, J. (1987b). A taxonomy of organizational justice theories. Academy of Management Review, 12, 9-22. Greenberg, J. (1987b). A taxonomy of organizational justice theories. Academy of Management Review, 12, 9-22. Greenberg, J. (1990a). Employee theft as a reaction to underpayment inequity: The hidden cost of pay cuts. Journal of Applied Psychol­ogy, 75, 561-568. Greenberg, J. (1990a). Employee theft as a reaction to underpayment inequity: The hidden cost of pay cuts. Journal of Applied Psychol­ogy, 75, 561-568. Greenberg, J. (1990b). Organizational justice: Yesterday, today, and to­morrow. Journal of Management, 16, 399-432. Greenberg, J. (1990b). Organizational justice: Yesterday, today, and to­morrow. Journal of Management, 16, 399-432. Greenberg, J. (1993a). The intellectual adolescence of organizational jus­tice: You’ve come a long way, maybe. Social Justice Research, 6, 135-148. Greenberg, J. (1993a). The intellectual adolescence of organizational jus­tice: You’ve come a long way, maybe. Social Justice Research, 6, 135-148. Greenberg, J. (1993b). The social side of fairness: Interpersonal and informational classes of organizational justice. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: Approaching fairness in human resource management (pp. 79-103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Greenberg, J. (1993b). The social side of fairness: Interpersonal and informational classes of organizational justice. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: Approaching fairness in human resource management (pp. 79-103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ♦Greenberg, J. (1993c). Stealing in the name of justice: Informational and interpersonal moderators of theft reactions to underpayment inequity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 81-103. ♦Greenberg, J. (1993c). Stealing in the name of justice: Informational and interpersonal moderators of theft reactions to underpayment inequity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 81-103. ♦Greenberg, J. (1994). Using socially fair treatment to promote acceptance of a work site smoking ban. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, ♦Greenberg, J. (1994). Using socially fair treatment to promote acceptance of a work site smoking ban. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 288­ 297. ♦Grienberger, ♦Grienberger, I. V., Rutte, C. G., & Van Knippenberg, A. F. Μ. (1997). Influence of social comparisons of outcomes and procedures on fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 913-919. ♦Griffeth, R. W., Vecchio, R. P., & Logan, J. W. (1989). Equity theory and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 394-401. ♦Griffeth, R. W., Vecchio, R. P., & Logan, J. W. (1989). Equity theory and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 394-401. ♦Hagedoom, Μ., Buunk, B. P., & Van de Vliert, E. (1998). Opening the black box between justice and reactions to unfavorable outcomes in the workplace. Social Justice Research, 11, 41-57. ♦Hagedoom, Μ., Buunk, B. P., & Van de Vliert, E. (1998). Opening the black box between justice and reactions to unfavorable outcomes in the workplace. Social Justice Research, 11, 41-57. ♦Hammer, T. H., Landau, J. C., & Stem, R. N. (1981). Absenteeism when workers have a voice: The case of employee ownership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 561-573. ♦Hammer, T. H., Landau, J. C., & Stem, R. N. (1981). Absenteeism when workers have a voice: The case of employee ownership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 561-573. ♦Harrison, D. A., McLaughlin, Μ. E., & Coalter, T. Μ. (1996). Context, cognition, and common method variance: Psychometric and verbal pro­tocol evidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro­cesses, 68, 246-261. ♦Harrison, D. A., McLaughlin, Μ. E., & Coalter, T. Μ. (1996). Context, cognition, and common method variance: Psychometric and verbal pro­tocol evidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro­cesses, 68, 246-261. Hom, P. W., Caranikas-Walker, F„ Prussia, G. E., & Griffeth, R. W. (1992). A meta-analytical structural equations analysis of a model of employee turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 890-909. Hom, P. W., Caranikas-Walker, F„ Prussia, G. E., & Griffeth, R. W. (1992). A meta-analytical structural equations analysis of a model of employee turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 890-909. ♦Hom, P. W„ Griffeth, R. W„ & ♦Hom, P. W„ Griffeth, R. W„ & Sellare, C. L. (1984). The validity of Mobley’s (1977) model of employee turnover. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 34, 141-174. Homans, G. (1961). Social behaviour: Its elementary forms. London: Routledge & Homans, G. (1961). Social behaviour: Its elementary forms. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. ♦Jones, ♦Jones, F. F., Scarpello, V., & Bergmann, T. (1999). Pay procedures: What makes them fair? Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol­ogy, 72, 129-145. ♦Joy, V. L., & Witt, L. A. (1992). Delay of gratification as a moderator of the procedural justice-distributive justice relationship. Group and Orga­nization Management, 17, 297-308. ♦Joy, V. L., & Witt, L. A. (1992). Delay of gratification as a moderator of the procedural justice-distributive justice relationship. Group and Orga­nization Management, 17, 297-308. ♦Kanfer, R., Sawyer, J. E., Earley, ♦Kanfer, R., Sawyer, J. E., Earley, P. C., & Lind, E. A. (1987). Fairness and participation in evaluation procedures: Effects on task attitudes and performance. Social Justice Research, I, 235-249. *Karambayya, R., & Brett, J. Μ. (1989). Managers handling disputes: Third-party roles and perceptions of fairness. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 687-704. *Karambayya, R., & Brett, J. Μ. (1989). Managers handling disputes: Third-party roles and perceptions of fairness. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 687-704. 442 442 442 442 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG *Keller, T., & Dansereau, F. (1995). Leadership and empowerment: A social exchange perspective. Human Relations, 48, 127-146. *Keller, T., & Dansereau, F. (1995). Leadership and empowerment: A social exchange perspective. Human Relations, 48, 127-146. *Kidwell, R. E., & Bennett, N. (1994). Employee reactions to electronic control systems: The role of procedural fairness. Group and Organiza­tion Management, 19, 203-218. *Kidwell, R. E., & Bennett, N. (1994). Employee reactions to electronic control systems: The role of procedural fairness. Group and Organiza­tion Management, 19, 203-218. *Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1993). Procedural justice, attitudes, and subsidiary top management compliance with multinationals’ corpo­rate strategic decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 502-526. *Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1993). Procedural justice, attitudes, and subsidiary top management compliance with multinationals’ corpo­rate strategic decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 502-526. *Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1996). Procedural justice and manag­ers’ in-role and extra-role behavior: The case of the multinational. Management Science, 42, 499-515. *Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1996). Procedural justice and manag­ers’ in-role and extra-role behavior: The case of the multinational. Management Science, 42, 499-515. *King, W. C., Jr., Miles, E. W., & Day, D. D. (1993). A test and refinement of the equity sensitivity construct. Journal of Organizational Behav­ior, 14, 301-317. *King, W. C., Jr., Miles, E. W., & Day, D. D. (1993). A test and refinement of the equity sensitivity construct. Journal of Organizational Behav­ior, 14, 301-317. ♦Konovsky, Μ. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1991). Perceived fairness of em­ployee drug testing as a predictor of employee attitudes and job perfor­mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 698-707. ♦Konovsky, Μ. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1991). Perceived fairness of em­ployee drug testing as a predictor of employee attitudes and job perfor­mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 698-707. ♦Konovsky, M. A., & Folger, R. (1991). The effects of procedures, social accounts, and benefits level on victims’ layoff reactions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 630—650. ♦Konovsky, M. A., & Folger, R. (1991). The effects of procedures, social accounts, and benefits level on victims’ layoff reactions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 630—650. *Konovsky, Μ. A., Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1987). Relative effects of procedural and distributive justice on employee attitudes. Represen­tative Research in Social Psychology, 17, 15-24. *Konovsky, Μ. A., Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1987). Relative effects of procedural and distributive justice on employee attitudes. Represen­tative Research in Social Psychology, 17, 15-24. *Konovsky, Μ. A., & Pugh, *Konovsky, Μ. A., & Pugh, S. D. (1994). Citizenship behavior and social exchange. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 656-669. *Korsgaard, Μ. A., & Roberson, L. (1995). Procedural justice in perfor­mance evaluation: The role of instrumental and non-instrumental voice in performance appraisal discussions. Journal of Management, 21, *Korsgaard, Μ. A., & Roberson, L. (1995). Procedural justice in perfor­mance evaluation: The role of instrumental and non-instrumental voice in performance appraisal discussions. Journal of Management, 21, 657­ 669. *Korsgaard, M. A., Roberson, L., & Rymph, R. D. (1998). What motivates fairness? The role of subordinate assertive behavior on managers’ inter­actional fairness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 731-744. *Korsgaard, M. A., Roberson, L., & Rymph, R. D. (1998). What motivates fairness? The role of subordinate assertive behavior on managers’ inter­actional fairness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 731-744. *Korsgaard, Μ. A., Schweiger, D. Μ., & *Korsgaard, Μ. A., Schweiger, D. Μ., & Sapienza, H. J. (1995). Building commitment, attachment, and trust in strategic decision-making teams: The role of procedural justice. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 60-84. *Latack, J. C., Josephs, S. L., Roach, B. L., & Levine, M. D. (1987). Carpenter apprentices: Comparison of career transitions for men and women. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 393-400. *Latack, J. C., Josephs, S. L., Roach, B. L., & Levine, M. D. (1987). Carpenter apprentices: Comparison of career transitions for men and women. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 393-400. *LaTour, S. (1978). Determinants of participant and observer satisfaction with adversary and inquisitorial modes of adjudication. Journal of Per­sonality and Social Psychology, 12, 1531—1545. *LaTour, S. (1978). Determinants of participant and observer satisfaction with adversary and inquisitorial modes of adjudication. Journal of Per­sonality and Social Psychology, 12, 1531—1545. *Lawson, M. B., & Angle, H. L. (1998). Upon reflection: Commitment, satisfaction, and regret after a corporate relocation. Group and Organi­zation Management, 23, 289-317. *Lawson, M. B., & Angle, H. L. (1998). Upon reflection: Commitment, satisfaction, and regret after a corporate relocation. Group and Organi­zation Management, 23, 289-317. *Lee, C., & Farh, J. L. (1999). The effects of gender in organizational justice perception. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 133-143. *Lee, C., & Farh, J. L. (1999). The effects of gender in organizational justice perception. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 133-143. *Leung, K., & *Leung, K., & Li, W. (1990). Psychological mechanisms of process­control effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 613-620. ♦Leventhal, G. S. (1976). The distribution of rewards and resources in groups and organizations. In L. Berkowitz & W. ♦Leventhal, G. S. (1976). The distribution of rewards and resources in groups and organizations. In L. Berkowitz & W. Walster (Eds.), Ad­vances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 91-131). New York: Academic Press. Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. Gergen, M. Greenberg, & R. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27-55). New York: Plenum. Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. Gergen, M. Greenberg, & R. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27-55). New York: Plenum. Leventhal, Leventhal, G. S., Karuža, J., & Fry, W. R. (1980). Beyond fairness: A theory of allocation preferences. In G. Mikula (Ed.), Justice and social interaction (pp. 167-218). New York: Springer-Verlag. *Levy, P. E., & Williams, J. R. (1998). The role of perceived system knowledge in predicting appraisal reactions, job satisfaction, and orga­nizational commitment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 53-65. *Levy, P. E., & Williams, J. R. (1998). The role of perceived system knowledge in predicting appraisal reactions, job satisfaction, and orga­nizational commitment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 53-65. ♦Lind, E. A., Kulik, ♦Lind, E. A., Kulik, C. T., Ambrose, M. A., & de Vera Park, M. V. (1993). Individual and corporate dispute resolution: Using procedural fairness as a decision heuristic. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 224-251. ♦Lind, E. A., Lissak, ♦Lind, E. A., Lissak, R. I., & Conlon, D. F. (1983). Decision control and process control effects on procedural fairness judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 338-350. Lind, E. A., & Tyler, Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum. ♦Lind, E. A., Tyler, T. R., & Huo, ♦Lind, E. A., Tyler, T. R., & Huo, Y. J. (1997). Procedural context and culture: Variation in the antecedents of procedural justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 767-780. ♦Lowe, ♦Lowe, R. H., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1995). A field study of distributive and procedural justice as predictors of satisfaction and organizational com­mitment. Journal of Business and Psychology, 10, 99-114. *Macan, *Macan, T. H., Avedon, M. J., Paese, Μ., & Smith, D. B. (1994). The effects of applicants’ reactions to cognitive ability tests and an assess­ment center. Personnel Psychology, 47, 715-738. ♦Magner, ♦Magner, N. R., Rahman, Μ., & Welker, R. B. (1996). The interactive effect of outcome favorability and procedural justice in work resource allocation on work performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychol­ogy, 26, 825-842. ♦Mansour-Cole, D. Μ., & Scott, ♦Mansour-Cole, D. Μ., & Scott, S. G. (1998). Hearing it through the grapevine: The influence of source, leader-relations, and legitimacy on survivors’ fairness perceptions. Personnel Psychology, 51, 25-54. ♦Mark, Μ. Μ. (1985). Expectations, procedural justice, and alternative reactions to being deprived of a desired outcome. Journal of Experi­mental Social Psychology, 21, 114-137. ♦Mark, Μ. Μ. (1985). Expectations, procedural justice, and alternative reactions to being deprived of a desired outcome. Journal of Experi­mental Social Psychology, 21, 114-137. ♦Martin, C. ♦Martin, C. L., & Bennett, N. (1996). The role of justice judgments in explaining the relationship between job satisfaction and organizational commitment. Group and Organization Management, 21, 84-104. ♦Martin, ♦Martin, C. L., Parsons, C. K., & Bennett, N. (1995). The influence of employee involvement program membership during downsizing: Atti­tudes toward the employer and the union. Journal of Management, 21, 879-890. ♦Martin, J. E., & Peterson, Μ. Μ. (1987). Two-tier wage structures: Implications for equity theory. Academy of Management Journal, 30, 297-315. ♦Martin, J. E., & Peterson, Μ. Μ. (1987). Two-tier wage structures: Implications for equity theory. Academy of Management Journal, 30, 297-315. ♦Martocchia, J. J., & Judge, T. A. (1995). When we don’t see eye to eye: Discrepancies between supervisors and subordinates in absence disci­plinary procedures. Journal of Management, 21, 251-278. ♦Martocchia, J. J., & Judge, T. A. (1995). When we don’t see eye to eye: Discrepancies between supervisors and subordinates in absence disci­plinary procedures. Journal of Management, 21, 251-278. Masterson, Masterson, S. S., Bartol, К. Μ., & Moye, N. (2000, April). Interactional and procedural justice: Type versus source of fairness. Paper presented at the 15th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Orga­nizational Psychology, New Orleans, LA. ♦Masterson, ♦Masterson, S. S., Lewis, K., Goldman, B. Μ., & Taylor, M. S. (2000). Integrating justice and social exchange: The differing effects of fair procedures and treatment on work relationships. Academy of Manage­ment Journal, 43, 738-748. Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1990). A review and meta-analysis of the antecedents, correlates, and consequences of organizational commit­ment. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 172-195. Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1990). A review and meta-analysis of the antecedents, correlates, and consequences of organizational commit­ment. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 172-195. ♦McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, ♦McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, P. D. (1992). Distributive and procedural justice as predictors of satisfaction with personal and organizational outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 626-637. ♦McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, ♦McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, P. D. (1996). Does having a say matter only if you get your way? Instrumental and value-expressive effects of employee voice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18, 289-303. ♦Miles, J. A., & Klein, H. J. (1998). The fairness of assigning group members to tasks. Group and Organization Management, 23, 71-96. ♦Miles, J. A., & Klein, H. J. (1998). The fairness of assigning group members to tasks. Group and Organization Management, 23, 71-96. ♦Miller, ♦Miller, M. L. (1989). Process and decision control in the work place: Separate effects, independence from distributive justice, and tests of explanatory mechanisms. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 10, 337-354. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 443 443 *Moorman, R. H. (1991). Relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 845-855. *Moorman, R. H. (1991). Relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 845-855. *Moorman, R. H., & Blakely, G. L. (1995). Individualism-collectivism as an individual difference predictor of organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 16, 127-142. *Moorman, R. H., & Blakely, G. L. (1995). Individualism-collectivism as an individual difference predictor of organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 16, 127-142. *Moorman, R. H., Blakely, G. L., & *Moorman, R. H., Blakely, G. L., & Niehoff, B. P. (1998). Does perceived organizational support mediate the relationship between procedural jus­tice and organizational citizenship behavior? Academy of Management Journal, 41, 351-357. *Moorman, *Moorman, R. H., Niehoff, B. P., & Organ, D. W. (1993). Treating employees fairly and organizational citizenship behavior: Sorting the effects of job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and procedural justice. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 6, 209-225. *Mossholder, K. W., Bennett, N., Kemery, E. R., & Wesolowski, M. A. (1998). Relationships between bases of power and work reactions: The mediational role of procedural justice. Journal of Management, 24, 533-552. *Mossholder, K. W., Bennett, N., Kemery, E. R., & Wesolowski, M. A. (1998). Relationships between bases of power and work reactions: The mediational role of procedural justice. Journal of Management, 24, 533-552. *Mossholder, K. W., Bennett, N., & Martin, *Mossholder, K. W., Bennett, N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). A multilevel analysis of procedural justice context. Journal of Organizational Behav­ior, 19, 131-141. *Moye, N. A., Masterson, *Moye, N. A., Masterson, S. S., & Bartol, K. M. (1997, August). Differ­entiating antecedents and consequences of procedural and interactional justice: Empirical evidence in support of separate constructs. Paper presented at the 56th Annual Meeting of the National Academy of Management, Boston, MA. *Newman, *Newman, K. L. (1993). Procedural justice and ethical decision making. Social Justice Research, 6, 113-134. *Niehoff, *Niehoff, B. P., & Moorman, R. H. (1993). Justice as a mediator of the relationship between methods of monitoring and organizational citizen­ship behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 527-556. *Ogilvie. J. R. (1986). The role of human resource management practices in predicting organizational commitment. Group and Organization Stud­ies, 11, 335-359. *Ogilvie. J. R. (1986). The role of human resource management practices in predicting organizational commitment. Group and Organization Stud­ies, 11, 335-359. *Olson-Buchanan, J. B. (1996). Voicing discontent: What happens to the grievance filer after the grievance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 52-63. *Olson-Buchanan, J. B. (1996). Voicing discontent: What happens to the grievance filer after the grievance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 52-63. Organ, D. W. (1990). The motivational basis of organizational citizenship behavior. In Organ, D. W. (1990). The motivational basis of organizational citizenship behavior. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in orga­nizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 43-72). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. *Parker, *Parker, C. P., Baltes, B. B., & Christiansen, N. D. (1997). Support for affirmative action, justice perceptions, and work attitudes: A study of gender and racial-ethnic group differences. Journal of Applied Psychol­ogy, 82. 376-389. Pillutla, Μ., & Murnighan, J. K. (1999). On fairness and justice in bargaining and in allocation decisions and procedures. Unpublished manuscript. Pillutla, Μ., & Murnighan, J. K. (1999). On fairness and justice in bargaining and in allocation decisions and procedures. Unpublished manuscript. *Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. Μ. (1997). Toward an explanation of applicant reactions: An examination of organizational justice and attri­bution frameworks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro­cesses, 72, 308 -335. *Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. Μ. (1997). Toward an explanation of applicant reactions: An examination of organizational justice and attri­bution frameworks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro­cesses, 72, 308 -335. *Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. M. (1998). Applicants’ reactions to the fairness of selection procedures: The effects of positive rule violations and time of measurement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 3-16. *Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. M. (1998). Applicants’ reactions to the fairness of selection procedures: The effects of positive rule violations and time of measurement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 3-16. *Ployhart, *Ployhart, R. E., Ryan, A. Μ., & Bennett, Μ. (1999). Explanations for selection decisions: Applicants’ reactions to informational and sensitiv­ity features of explanations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 87-106. Podsakoff, P. Μ., MacKenzie, S. B„ & Bommer, Podsakoff, P. Μ., MacKenzie, S. B„ & Bommer, W. H. (1996). Meta­analysis of the relationships between Kerr and Jermier's substitutes for leadership and employee job attitudes, role perceptions, and perfor- manee. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81. 380-399. *Pond, *Pond, S. B., III, Nacoste, R. W., Mohr, M. F., & Rodriguez, C. M. (1997). The measurement of organizational citizenship behavior: Are we assum­ing too much? Journal of Applied Psychology, 27, 1527-1554. The measurement of organizational citizenship behavior: Are we assum­ing too much? Journal of Applied Psychology, 27, 1527-1554. *Porter, C. *Porter, C. O. L. H., Conlon, D. E„ & Barber, A. E. (1999, August). Effects of justice in salary negotiations on recruitment outcomes: A laboratory experiment. Paper presented at the 58th Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, Chicago, IL. *Rasinski, D. A. (1990). Contextualizing procedural fairness: Factors mediating the effect of process control on leadership evaluation in organizations. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 459-477. *Rasinski, D. A. (1990). Contextualizing procedural fairness: Factors mediating the effect of process control on leadership evaluation in organizations. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 459-477. Reichers, A. E., & Schneider, B. (1990). Climate and culture: Life cycles of constructs. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture (pp. 5-39). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Reichers, A. E., & Schneider, B. (1990). Climate and culture: Life cycles of constructs. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture (pp. 5-39). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. *Richard, *Richard, O. C., & Kirby, S. L. (1998). Women recruits’ perceptions of workforce diversity program selection decisions: A procedural justice examination. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 183-188. Ryan, A. (1993). Justice. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Ryan, A. (1993). Justice. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. *Sapienza, H. J., & Korsgaard, M. A. (1996). Procedural justice in entrepreneur-investor relations. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 544-574. *Sapienza, H. J., & Korsgaard, M. A. (1996). Procedural justice in entrepreneur-investor relations. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 544-574. *Scandura, T. A. (1997). Mentoring and organizational justice: An empir­ical investigation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 51, 58-69. *Scandura, T. A. (1997). Mentoring and organizational justice: An empir­ical investigation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 51, 58-69. *Scarpello, V., & Jones, *Scarpello, V., & Jones, F. F. (1996). Why justice matters in compensation decision making. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17, 285-299. *Schappe, *Schappe, S. P. (1998). The influence of job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and fairness perceptions on organizational citizenship be­havior. Journal of Psychology, 132, 277-290. *Schaubroeck, J., May, D. R., & Brown, F. W. (1994). Procedural justice explanations and employee reactions to economic hardship: A field experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 455-460. *Schaubroeck, J., May, D. R., & Brown, F. W. (1994). Procedural justice explanations and employee reactions to economic hardship: A field experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 455-460. *Scher, S. J. (1997). Measuring the consequences of injustice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 482-497. *Scher, S. J. (1997). Measuring the consequences of injustice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 482-497. *Schmidt, *Schmidt, F. L., Hunter, J. E., & Outerbridge, A. N. (1986). Impact of job experience and ability on job knowledge, work sample performance, and supervisory ratings of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychol­ogy, 71, 432-439. *Schminke, Μ., Ambrose, M. L., & Noel, T. W. (1997). The effect of ethical frameworks on perceptions of organizational justice. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 1190-1207. *Schminke, Μ., Ambrose, M. L., & Noel, T. W. (1997). The effect of ethical frameworks on perceptions of organizational justice. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 1190-1207. *Schmitt, Μ. (1996). Individual differences in sensitivity to befallen in­justice (SB1). Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 3-20. *Schmitt, Μ. (1996). Individual differences in sensitivity to befallen in­justice (SB1). Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 3-20. *Shapiro, *Shapiro, D. L. (1991). The effects of explanations on negative reactions to deceit. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 614-630. *Shapiro, D. L., & Brett, J. Μ. (1993). Comparing three processes under­lying judgments of procedural justice: A field study of mediation and arbitration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1 *Shapiro, D. L., & Brett, J. Μ. (1993). Comparing three processes under­lying judgments of procedural justice: A field study of mediation and arbitration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1 167­ 1177. *Shapiro, D. L„ Buttner, *Shapiro, D. L„ Buttner, E. H., & Barry, B. (1994). Explanations: What factors enhance their perceived adequacy? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58, 346-368. *Skarlicki, D. P., Ellard, J. H., & Kelln, *Skarlicki, D. P., Ellard, J. H., & Kelln, B. R. C. (1998). Third-party perceptions of a layoff: Procedural, derogation, and retributive aspects of justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 119-127. *Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. (1997). Retaliation in the workplace: The roles of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 434 -443. *Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. (1997). Retaliation in the workplace: The roles of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 434 -443. *Skarlicki, *Skarlicki, D. P., Folger, R., & Tesluk, P. (1999). Personality as a mod­erator in the relationship between fairness and retaliation. Academy of Management Journal, 42, 100—108. *Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, *Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (1996). Increasing citizenship behavior within a labor union: A test of organizational justice theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 161-169. *Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, *Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (1997). Leadership training in organi- 444 444 444 444 COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG COLQUITT, CONLON, WESSON, PORTER, AND NG zational justice to increase citizenship behavior within a labor union: A replication. Personnel Psychology, 50, 617-633. zational justice to increase citizenship behavior within a labor union: A replication. Personnel Psychology, 50, 617-633. *Smith, H. J., & Tyler, T. R. (1996). Justice and power: When will justice concerns encourage the advantaged to support policies which redistrib­ute economic resources and the disadvantaged to willingly obey the law? European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 171-200. *Smith, H. J., & Tyler, T. R. (1996). Justice and power: When will justice concerns encourage the advantaged to support policies which redistrib­ute economic resources and the disadvantaged to willingly obey the law? European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 171-200. ♦Smith, H. J., Tyler, T. R., Huo, Y. J., Ortiz, D. J., & Lind, E. A. (1998). The self-relevant implications of the group-value model: Group mem­bership, self-worth, and treatment quality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 470-493. ♦Smith, H. J., Tyler, T. R., Huo, Y. J., Ortiz, D. J., & Lind, E. A. (1998). The self-relevant implications of the group-value model: Group mem­bership, self-worth, and treatment quality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 470-493. *Smither, J. W., Reilly, R. R., Millsap, R. E., Pearlman, K., & Stoffey, R. W. (1993). Applicant reactions to selection procedures. Personnel Psychology, 46, 49-76. *Smither, J. W., Reilly, R. R., Millsap, R. E., Pearlman, K., & Stoffey, R. W. (1993). Applicant reactions to selection procedures. Personnel Psychology, 46, 49-76. *Spencer, D. G. (1986). Employee voice and employee retention. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 488-502. *Spencer, D. G. (1986). Employee voice and employee retention. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 488-502. *Sujak, *Sujak, D. A., Villanova, P., & Daly, J. P. (1995). The effects of drug­testing program characteristics on applicants’ attitudes toward potential employment. Journal of Psychology, 129, 401-416. ♦Summers, T. P., & DeNisi, A. S. (1990). In search of Adams’ other: Reexamination of referents used in the evaluation of pay. Human Rela­tions, 43, 497-511. ♦Summers, T. P., & DeNisi, A. S. (1990). In search of Adams’ other: Reexamination of referents used in the evaluation of pay. Human Rela­tions, 43, 497-511. ♦Sweeney, P. D. (1990). Distributive justice and pay satisfaction: A field test of an equity theory prediction. Journal of Business and Psychol­ogy, 4, 329-341. ♦Sweeney, P. D. (1990). Distributive justice and pay satisfaction: A field test of an equity theory prediction. Journal of Business and Psychol­ogy, 4, 329-341. ♦Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, ♦Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, D. B. (1993). Workers’ evaluation of the “ends” and the “means”: An examination of four models of distributive and procedural justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 23-40. ♦Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, ♦Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, D. B. (1997). Process and outcome: Gender differences in the assessment of justice. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 83-98. ♦Sweeney, P. D., McFarlin, ♦Sweeney, P. D., McFarlin, D. B., & Cotton, J. L. (1991). Locus of control as a moderator of the relationship between perceived influence and procedural justice. Human Relations, 44, 333-342. ♦Syroit, J., Lodewijkx, H., ♦Syroit, J., Lodewijkx, H., Franssen, E., & Gerstel, I. (1993). Organiza­tional commitment and satisfaction with work among transferred em­ployees: An application of referent cognitions theory. Social Justice Research, 6, 219-234. ♦Tansky, J. W. (1993). Justice and organizational citizenship behavior: What is the relationship? Employee Responsibilities and Rights Jour­nal, 6, 195-207. ♦Tansky, J. W. (1993). Justice and organizational citizenship behavior: What is the relationship? Employee Responsibilities and Rights Jour­nal, 6, 195-207. ♦Tata, J., & Bowes-Sperry, L. (1996). Emphasis on distributive, proce­dural, and interactional justice: Differential perceptions of men and women. Psychological Reports, 79, 1327-1330. ♦Tata, J., & Bowes-Sperry, L. (1996). Emphasis on distributive, proce­dural, and interactional justice: Differential perceptions of men and women. Psychological Reports, 79, 1327-1330. ♦Taylor, M. S., Tracy, K. B., Renard, M. K., Harrison, J. K., & Carroll, S. J. (1995). Due process in performance appraisal: A quasi-experiment in procedural justice. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 495-523. ♦Taylor, M. S., Tracy, K. B., Renard, M. K., Harrison, J. K., & Carroll, S. J. (1995). Due process in performance appraisal: A quasi-experiment in procedural justice. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 495-523. ♦Tepper, B. J., ♦Tepper, B. J., Eisenbach, R. A., Kirby, S. L., & Potter, P. W. (1998). Test of a justice based model of subordinates’ resistance to downward influ­ence attempts. Group and Organization Management, 23, 144-160. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ♦Tremblay, Μ., Sire, B., & ♦Tremblay, Μ., Sire, B., & Pelchat, A. (1998). A study of the determinants and of the impact of flexibility on employee benefit satisfaction. Human Relations, 51, 667-688. ♦Truxillo, D. Μ., & Bauer, T. N. (1999). Applicant reactions to test score banding in entry-level and promotional contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 322-339. ♦Truxillo, D. Μ., & Bauer, T. N. (1999). Applicant reactions to test score banding in entry-level and promotional contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 322-339. ♦Tyler, ♦Tyler, T. R. (1987). Conditions leading to value-expressive effects in judgments of procedural justice: A test of four models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 333-344. Tyler, Tyler, T. R. (1989). The psychology of procedural justice: A test of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 830-838. group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 830-838. Tyler, Tyler, T. R. (1990). Why people obey the law: Procedural justice, legiti­macy, and compliance. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Tyler, Tyler, T. R. (1994). Psychological models of the justice motive: Anteced­ents of distributive and procedural justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 850-863. Tyler, T. R., & Tyler, T. R., & Bies, R. J. (1990). Beyond formal procedures: The inter­personal context of procedural justice. In J. S. Carroll (Ed.), Applied social psychology and organizational settings (pp. 77-98). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ♦Tyler, T. R., & Caine, A. (1981). The influence of outcomes and proce­dures on satisfaction with formal leaders. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 642-655. ♦Tyler, T. R., & Caine, A. (1981). The influence of outcomes and proce­dures on satisfaction with formal leaders. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 642-655. ♦Tyler, ♦Tyler, T. R., & Degoey, P. (1995). Collective restraint in social dilemmas: Procedural justice and social identification effects on support for author­ities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 482-497. Tyler, T. R„ Degoey, P., & Smith, Tyler, T. R„ Degoey, P., & Smith, H. J. (1996). Understanding why the justice of group procedures matters: A test of the psychological dynam­ics of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­chology, 70, 913-930. Tyler, Tyler, T. R., Lind, E. A., Ohbuchi, K. I., Sugawara, I., & Huo, Y. J. (1998). Conflict with outsiders: Disputing within and across boundaries. Per­sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 137-146. ♦Tyler, ♦Tyler, T. R., Rasinski, K. A., & McGraw, K. M. (1985). The influence of perceived justice on the endorsement of political leaders. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 700-725. ♦Tyler, ♦Tyler, T. R., Rasinski, K. A., & Spodick, N. (1985). Influence of voice on satisfaction with leaders: Exploring the meaning of process control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 72-81. ♦Van den Bos, K., Lind, F. A., ♦Van den Bos, K., Lind, F. A., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). How do I judge my outcome when I do not know the outcome of others? The psychology of the fair process effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1034-1046. ♦Van den Bos, K., ♦Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1996). The consis­tency rule and the voice effect: The influence of expectations on proce­dural fairness judgments and performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 411-428. ♦Van den Bos, K., ♦Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). Procedural and distributive justice: What is fair depends more on what comes first than on what comes next. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 95-104. ♦Van den Bos, K., Wilke, H. A. Μ., & Lind, E. A. (1998). When do we need procedural fairness? The role of trust in authority. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1449-1458. ♦Van den Bos, K., Wilke, H. A. Μ., & Lind, E. A. (1998). When do we need procedural fairness? The role of trust in authority. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1449-1458. ♦Van Dierendonck, D., ♦Van Dierendonck, D., Schaufelt, W. B., & Buunk, B. P. (1998). The evaluation of an individual burnout intervention program: The role of inequity and social support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 392­ 407. ♦Vecchio, R. P., Griffeth, R. W., & Hom, P. W. (1986). The predictive utility of the vertical dyad linkage approach. Journal of Social Psychol­ogy, 126, 617-625. ♦Vecchio, R. P., Griffeth, R. W., & Hom, P. W. (1986). The predictive utility of the vertical dyad linkage approach. Journal of Social Psychol­ogy, 126, 617-625. ♦Vermunt, ♦Vermunt, R., Blaauw, F., & Lind, E. A. (1998). Fairness evaluations of encounters with police officers and correctional officers. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1107-1124. Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1995). Theory testing: Combining psy­chometric meta-analysis and structural equations modeling. Personnel Psychology, 48, 865- 885. ♦Wanberg, ♦Wanberg, C. R., Bunce, L. W., & Gavin, M. B. (1999). Perceived fairness of layoffs among individuals who have been laid off: A longitudinal study. Personnel Psychology, 52, 59 - 84. Wanous, J. P., Sullivan, Wanous, J. P., Sullivan, S. F., & Malinak, J. (1989). The role of judgment calls in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 259-264. META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE META-ANALYTIC REVIEW OF ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE 445 445 *Weaver, G. R., & Conlon, D. E. (1997, August). Explaining broken promises: Timing, justice effects, and behavioral outcomes. Paper pre­sented at the 57th Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, Boston, MA. *Weaver, G. R., & Conlon, D. E. (1997, August). Explaining broken promises: Timing, justice effects, and behavioral outcomes. Paper pre­sented at the 57th Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, Boston, MA. *Welbourne, T. Μ., *Welbourne, T. Μ., Balkin, D. B., & Gomez-Mejia, L. R. (1995). Gain­sharing and mutual monitoring: A combined agency-organizational jus­tice interpretation. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 881-899. ♦Wesolowski, Μ. A., & Mossholder, K. W. (1997). Relational demography in supervisor-subordinate dyads: Impact on subordinate job satisfaction, burnout, and perceived procedural justice. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 351-362. ♦Wesolowski, Μ. A., & Mossholder, K. W. (1997). Relational demography in supervisor-subordinate dyads: Impact on subordinate job satisfaction, burnout, and perceived procedural justice. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 351-362. ♦White, Μ. Μ., Tansky, J. A., & ♦White, Μ. Μ., Tansky, J. A., & Baik, К. (1995). Linking culture and perceptions of justice: A comparison of students in Virginia and South Korea. Psychological Reports, 77, 1103-1112. perceptions of justice: A comparison of students in Virginia and South Korea. Psychological Reports, 77, 1103-1112. Whitener, E. Μ. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and credibility intervals in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 315-321. Whitener, E. Μ. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and credibility intervals in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 315-321. Williams, Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and organiza­tional commitment as predictors of organizational citizenship and in-role behaviors. Journal of Management, 17, 601-617. Received August 23, 1999 Received August 23, 1999 Revision received May 12, 2000 Revision received May 12, 2000 Accepted May 15, 2000 ■ Accepted May 15, 2000 ■ Members of Underrepresented Groups: Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted Members of Underrepresented Groups: Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript re­viewers are vital to the publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate more in this process. If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write to If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write to Demarie Jackson at the address below. Please note the following important points: • • • To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective review. To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective review. • • To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical jour­nals that are most central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission within the context of existing re­search. To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical jour­nals that are most central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission within the context of existing re­search. • • To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information. Please include with your letter your vita. In your letter, please iden­tify which To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information. Please include with your letter your vita. In your letter, please iden­tify which APA journal(s) you are interested in, and describe your area of exper­tise. Be as specific as possible. For example, “social psychology” is not suffi­cient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude change” as well. • • Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1-4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript thoroughly. Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1-4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript thoroughly. Write to Write to Demarie Jackson, Journals Office, American Psychological Association, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242. Copyright of Copyright of Copyright of Journal of Applied Psychology is the property of American Psychological Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Calculate the price of your order

550 words
We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
Total price:
$26
The price is based on these factors:
Academic level
Number of pages
Urgency
Basic features
  • Free title page and bibliography
  • Unlimited revisions
  • Plagiarism-free guarantee
  • Money-back guarantee
  • 24/7 support
On-demand options
  • Writer’s samples
  • Part-by-part delivery
  • Overnight delivery
  • Copies of used sources
  • Expert Proofreading
Paper format
  • 275 words per page
  • 12 pt Arial/Times New Roman
  • Double line spacing
  • Any citation style (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard)

Our guarantees

Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.

Money-back guarantee

You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.

Read more

Zero-plagiarism guarantee

Each paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.

Read more

Free-revision policy

Thanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.

Read more

Confidentiality Guarantee

Your email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.

Read more

Fair-cooperation guarantee

By sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.

Read more

24/7 Support

Our specialists are always online to help you! We are available 24/7 via live chat, WhatsApp, and phone to answer questions, correct mistakes, or just address your academic fears.

See our T&Cs
Live Chat+1(978) 822-0999EmailWhatsApp

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code ESSAYHELP